The Rape of the Mind
A. M. Meerloo, M.D.
PART ONE
THE TECHNIQUES OF INDIVIDUAL SUBMISSION
The first part of this book is devoted to various techniques used to make man a
meek conformist. In addition to actual political occurrences, attention is called to
some ideas born in the laboratory and to the drug techniques that facilitate
brainwashing. The last chapter deals with the subtle psychological mechanisms of
mental submission.
CHAPTER ONE
YOU TOO WOULD CONFESS
A fantastic thing is happening in our world. Today a man is no longer punished only
for the crimes he has in fact committed. Now he may be compelled to confess to
crimes that have been conjured up by his judges, who use his confession for political
purposes. It is not enough for us to damn as evil those who sit in judgment. We must
understand what impels the false admission of guilt; we must take another look at
the human mind in all its frailty and vulnerability.
The Enforced Confession
During the Korean War, an officer of the United States Marine Corps, Colonel Frank
H. Schwable, was taken prisoner by the Chinese Communists. After months of
intense psychological pressure and physical degradation, he signed a well
documented "confession" that the United States was carrying on bacteriological
warfare against the enemy. The confession named names, cited missions, described
meetings and strategy conferences. This was a tremendously valuable propaganda
tool for the totalitarians. They cabled the news all over the world: "The United States
of America is fighting the peace loving people of China by dropping bombs loaded
with disease spreading bacteria, in violation of international law."
After his repatriation, Colonel Schwable issued a sworn statement repudiating his
confession, and describing his long months of imprisonment. Later, he was brought
before a military court of inquiry. He testified in his own defense before that court: "I
was never convinced in my own mind that we in the First Marine Air Wing had used
bug warfare. I knew we hadn't, but the rest of it was real to me the conferences, the
planes, and how they would go about their missions."
"The words were mine," the Colonel continued, "but the thoughts were theirs. That is
the hardest thing I have to explain: how a man can sit down and write something he
knows is false, and yet, to sense it, to feel it, to make it seem real."
This is the way Dr. Charles W. Mayo, a leading American physician and government
representative, explained brainwashing in an official statement before the United
Nations: "...the tortures used...although they include many brutal physical injuries,
are not like the medieval torture of the rack and the thumb screw. They are subtler,
more prolonged, and intended to be more terrible in their effect. They are calculated
to disintegrate the mind of an intelligent victim, to distort his sense of values, to a
point where he will not simply cry out 'I did it!' but will become a seemingly willing
accomplice to the complete disintegration of his integrity and the production of an
elaborate fiction."
The Schwable case is but one example of a defenceless prisoner being compelled to
tell a big lie. If we are to survive as free men, we must face up to this problem of
politically inspired mental coercion, with all its ramifications.
It is more than twenty years (in 1956) since psychologists first began to suspect that
the human mind can easily fall prey to dictatorial powers. In 1933, the German
Reichstag building was burned to the ground. The Nazis arrested a Dutchman,
Marinus Van der Lubbe, and accused him of the crime. Van der Lubbe was known
by Dutch psychiatrists to be mentally unstable. He had been a patient in a mental
institution in Holland. And his weakness and lack of mental balance became
apparent to the world when he appeared before the court. Wherever news of the trial
reached, men wondered: "Can that foolish little fellow be a heroic revolutionary, a
man who is willing to sacrifice his life to an ideal?"
During the court sessions Van der Lubbe was evasive, dull, and apathetic. Yet the
reports of the Dutch psychiatrists described him as a gay, alert, unstable character, a
man whose moods changed rapidly, who liked to vagabond around, and who had all
kinds of fantasies about changing the world.
On the forty second day of the trial, Van der Lubbe's behaviour changed
dramatically. His apathy disappeared. It became apparent that he had been quite
aware of everything that had gone on during the previous sessions. He criticized the
slow course of the procedure. He demanded punishment either by imprisonment or
death. He spoke about his "inner voices." He insisted that he had his moods in
check. Then he fell back into apathy. We now recognize these symptoms as a
combination of behaviour forms which we can call a confession syndrome. In 1933
this type of behaviour was unknown to psychiatrists. Unfortunately, it is very familiar
today and is frequently met in cases of extreme mental coercion.
Van der Lubbe was subsequently convicted and executed. When the trial was over,
the world began to realize that he had merely been a scapegoat. The Nazis
themselves had burned down the Reichstag building and had staged the crime and
the trial so that they could take over Germany. Still later we realized that Van der
Lubbe was the victim of a diabolically clever misuse of medical knowledge and
psychological technique, through which he had been transformed into a useful,
passive, meek automaton, who replied merely yes or no to his interrogators during
most of the court sessions. In a few moments he threatened to jump out of his
enforced role. Even at that time there were rumours that the man had been drugged
into submission, though we never became sure of that.
(NOTE: The psychiatric report about the case of Van der Lubbe is published by
Bonhoeffer and Zutt. Though they were unfamiliar with the "menticide syndrome,"
and not briefed by their political fuehrers, they give a good description about the
pathologic, apathetic behaviour, and his tremendous change of moods. They deny
the use of drugs.)
Between 1936 and 1938 the world became more conscious of the very real danger
of systematized mental coercion in the field of politics. This was the period of the well
remembered Moscow purge trials. It was almost impossible to believe that dedicated
old Bolsheviks, who had given their lives to a revolutionary movement, had suddenly
turned into dastardly traitors. When, one after another, everyone of the accused
confessed and beat his breast, the general reaction was that this was a great show
of deception, intended only as a propaganda move for the non Communist world.
Then it became apparent that a much worse tragedy was being enacted. The men
on trial had once been human beings. Now they were being systematically changed
into puppets. Their puppeteers called the tune and manipulated their actions. When,
from time to time, news came through showing how hard, rigid revolutionaries could
be changed into meek, self accusing sheep, all over the world the last remnants of
the belief in the free community presumably being built in Soviet Russia began to
crumble.
In recent years, the spectacle of confession to uncommitted crimes has become
more and more common. The list ranges from Communist through non Communist
to anti Communist, and includes men of such different types as the Czech Bolshevik
Rudolf Slansky and the Hungarian cardinal, Joseph Mindszenty.
Mental Coercion and Enemy Occupation
Those of us who lived in the Nazi occupied countries during the Second World War
learned to understand only too well how people could be forced into false
confessions, and into betrayals of those they loved. I myself was born in the
Netherlands and lived there until the Nazi occupation forced me to flee. In the early
days of the occupation, when we heard the first eyewitness descriptions of what
happened during Nazi interrogations of captured resistance workers, we were
frightened and alarmed.
The first aim of the Gestapo was to force prisoners under torture to betray their
friends and to report new victims for further torture. The Brown Shirts demanded
names and more names, not bothering to ascertain whether or not they were given
falsely under the stress of terror. I remember very clearly one meeting held by a
small group of resisters to discuss the growing fear and insecurity. Everybody at that
meeting could expect to be mentioned and picked up by the Gestapo at some time.
Should we be able to stand the Nazi treatment, or would we also be forced to
become informers? This question was being asked by anti Nazis in all the occupied
countries.
During the second year of the occupation we realized that it was better not to be in
touch with one another. More than two contacts were unsafe. We tried to find
medical and psychiatric preventives to harden us against the Nazi torture we
expected. As a matter of fact, I myself conducted some experiments to determine
whether or not narcotics would harden us against pain. However, the results were
paradoxical. Narcotics can create pain insensitivity, but their dulling action at the
same time makes people more vulnerable to mental pressure. Even at that time we
knew, as did the Nazis themselves, that it was not the direct physical pain that broke
people, but the continuous humiliation and mental torture. One of my patients, who
was subjected to such an interrogation, managed to remain silent. He refused to
answer a single question, and finally the Nazis dismissed him. But he never
recovered from this terrifying experience. He hardly spoken even when he returned
home. He simply sat bitter, full of indignation and in a few weeks he died. It was not
his physical wounds that had killed him; it was the combination of fear and wounded
pride.
We held many discussions about ways of strengthening our captured underground
workers or preventing them from final self betrayal. Should some of our people be
given suicide capsules? That could only be a last resort. Narcotics like morphine give
only a temporary anaesthesia and relief; moreover, the enemy would certainly find
the capsules and take them away.
We had heard about German attempts to give cocaine and amphetamine to their air
pilots for use in combat exhaustion, but neither medicament was reliable. Those
drugs might revive the body by making it less sensitive to pain, but at the same time
they dulled the mind. If captured members of the underground were to take them, as
experiments had shown, their bodies might not feel the effects of physical torture, but
their hazy minds might turn them into easier dupes of the Nazis.
We also tried systematic exercises in mental relaxation and auto hypnosis
(comparable with Yogi exercises) in order to make the body more insensitive to
hunger and pain. If an individual's attention is fixed on the development of conscious
awareness of automatic body functions, such as breathing, the alert functioning of
the brain cortex can be reduced, and awareness of pain will diminish. This state of
pain insensitivity can sometimes be achieved through autohypnotic exercises. But
very few of our people were able to bring themselves into such anaesthesia.
Finally we evolved this simple psychological trick: when you can no longer outwit the
enemy or resist talking, the best thing to do is to talk too much. This was the idea:
keep yourself sullen and act the fool; play the coward and confess more than there is
to confess. Later we were able to verify that this method was successful in several
cases. Scatterbrained simpletons confused the enemy much more than silent heroes
whose stamina was finally undermined in spite of everything.
I had to flee Holland after a policeman warned me that my name had been
mentioned in an interrogation. I had twice been questioned by the Nazis on minor
matters and without bodily torture. When they later caught up with me in Belgium,
probably as the result of a betrayal, I had to undergo a long initial examination in
which I was beaten, fortunately not too seriously. The interview had started
pleasantly enough. Apparently, the Nazi officer in charge thought he would be able
to get information out of me through friendly methods. Indeed, we even had a
discussion (since I am a psychiatrist) about the methods used in interrogation. But
the officer's mood changed, and he behaved with all the sadistic characteristics we
had come to expect from his type. Happily, I managed to escape from Belgium that
very night before a more systematic and more torturous investigation could begin.
Arriving at the London headquarters after an adventurous trip through France and
Spain, I became Chief of the Psychological Department of the Netherlands Forces in
England. In this official position I was able to gather data on what was happening to
the millions of victims of Nazi terror and torture. Later on I questioned and treated
several escapees from internment and concentration camps. These people had
become real experts in suffering. The variety of human reactions under those
infernal circumstances taught us an ugly truth: the spirit of most men can be broken;
men can be reduced to the level of animal behaviour. Both torturer and victim finally
lose all human dignity.
My government gave me the power to investigate a group of traitors and I also
interrogated imprisoned Nazis. When I reviewed all these wartime experiences, all
the confusion about courage and cowardice, treason, morale, and mental fortitude, I
must confess that my eyes were only really opened after a study of the Nuremberg
trials of the Nazi leaders. These trials gave us the real story of the systematic
coercive methods used by th e Nazis. At about the same time we began to learn
more about the perverted psychological strategy Russia and her satellites were
using.
Witchcraft and Torture
The specific techniques used in the modern world to break man's mind and will to
extort confessions for political propaganda purposes are relatively new (in 1956) and
highly refined. Yet enforced confession itself is nothing new. From time immemorial
tyrants and dictators have needed these "voluntary" confessions to justify their own
evil deeds. The knowledge that the human mind can be influenced, tamed, and
broken down into servility is far older than the modern dictatorial concept of enforced
indoctrination.
The primitive shaman used awe inspiring ritual to bring his victim into such a state of
fright hypnosis that he yielded to all suggestions. The native on whom a spell of
doom has been cast by the medicine man may become so hypnotized by his own
fear that he simply sits down, accepts his fate, and dies (Malinowski).
Throughout history men have had an intuitive understanding that the mind can be
manipulated. Elaborate strategies have been worked out to achieve this end.
Ecstasy rituals, frightening masks, loud noises, eerie chants all have been used to
compel the crowd to accept the beliefs of their leaders. Even if an ordinary man at
first resists a cruel shaman or medicine man, the hypnotizing ritual gradually breaks
his will.
More painful methods are not new either. When we study the old reports of the
Inquisition, or of the many witch trials, both in Europe and America, we learn a great
deal about these methods. The floating test is one example. Those accused of
witchcraft were thrown into the river, their feet and hands tied together. If the body
did not sink, the victim was immediately pulled out of the water and burned at the
stake. The fact that he did not sink was proof positive of his guilt. If, on the other
hand, the accused obeyed the law of gravity and sank to the bottom of the river, the
drowned body was ceremoniously removed from the river and proclaimed innocent.
Not much choice was left to the victim!
Man has been tremendously inventive in developing means for inflicting suffering on
his fellow man. With refined passion he has devised techniques which provoke the
most exquisite pain in the most vulnerable parts of the human body. The rack and
the thumbscrew are age old instruments and have been used not only by primitive
judges but also by so called civilized dictators and tyrants.
In order better to understand modern mental torture, we must constantly keep in
mind the fact that from the earliest days bodily anguish and the rack were never
meant merely to inflict pain on the victim. They may not have expressed their
understanding in sophisticated terms, but the medieval judge and hangman were
nevertheless aware that there is a peculiar spiritual relationship and mental interplay
between the victim and the rest of the community.
Much painful torture and hanging had to be done as public demonstrations. After
suffering the most intense pain, the witch would not only confess to shocking sexual
debaucheries with the devil, but would herself gradually come to believe the stories
she had invented and would die convinced of her guilt. The whole ritual of
interrogation and torture finally compelled her to yield to the fantasies of her judges
and accusers. In the end she even yearned for death. She wanted to be burned at
the stake in order to exorcise the devil and expiate her sins.
These same judges and hangman realized, too, that their witch trials were intended
not only to torture the witches, but even more to torture the bystanders, who, albeit
unconsciously, identified themselves with the victims. This is, of course, one of the
reasons burnings and hangings were held in public and became the occasion for
great pageants. Terror thus became widespread, and many judges spoke
euphemistically of the preventive action of such torture. Psychologically, we can see
this entire device as a blackmailing of human sympathy and the general tendency to
identify with others.
As far back as 1563 the courageous Dutch physician Johannes Wier published his
masterwork, De Praestigiis Daemonum (On the Delusions About Demons) in which
he states that the collective and voluntary self accusation of older women through
which they exposed themselves to torture and death by their inquisitors was in itself
an act inspired by the devil, a trick of demons, whose aim it was to doom not only the
innocent women but also their reckless judges. Wier was the first medical man to
introduce what became the psychiatric concept of DELUSION and mental blindness.
Wherever his book had influence, the persecution of witches ceased, in some
countries more than one hundred and fifty years before it was finally brought to an
end throughout the civilized world. His work and his insights became one of the main
instruments for fighting the witch delusion and physical torture (Baschwitz). Wier
realized even then that witches were scapegoats for the inner confusion and
desperation of their judges and of the "Zeitgeist" in general.
The Refinement of the Rack
All knowledge can be used either for good or for evil, and psychology is not immune
to this general law. Psychology has delivered up to man new means of torture and
intrusion into the mind. We must be more and more aware of what these methods
and techniques are if we are successfully to fight them. They can often be more
painful and mentally more paralyzing than the rack. Strong personalities can tolerate
physical agony; often it serves to increase stubborn resistance. No matter what the
constitution of the victim, physical torture finally leads to a protective loss of
consciousness. But to withstand mental torture leading to creeping mental
breakdown demands an even stronger personality.
What we call brainwashing (a word derived from the Chinese "Hsi Nao") is an
elaborate ritual of systematic indoctrination, conversion, and self accusation used to
change non Communists into submissive followers of the party (Hunter). "Menticide"
is a word coined by me and derived from "mens", the mind, and "caedere", to kill.
(NOTE: Here I followed the etymology used by the United Nations to form the word
"genocide," meaning the systematic destruction of racial groups.)
Both words indicate the same perverted refinement of the rack, putting it on what
appears to be a more acceptable level. But it is a thousand times worse and a
thousand times more useful to the inquisitor.
Menticide is an old crime against the human mind and spirit but systematized anew.
It is an organized system of psychological intervention and judicial perversion
through which a powerful dictator can imprint his own opportunistic thoughts upon
the minds of those he plans to use and destroy. The terrorized victims finally find
themselves compelled to express complete conformity to the tyrant's wishes.
Through court procedures, at which the victim mechanically reels off an inner record
which has been prepared by his inquisitors during a preceding period, public opinion
is lulled and thrown off guard. "A real traitor has been punished," people think. "The
man has confessed!" His confession can be used for propaganda, for the cold war,
to instil fear and terror, to accuse the enemy falsely, or to exercise a constant mental
pressure upon others.
One important result of this procedure is the great confusion it creates in the mind of
every observer, friend or foe. In the end no one knows how to distinguish truth from
falsehood. The totalitarian potentate, in order to break down the minds of men, first
needs widespread mental chaos and verbal confusion, because both paralyze his
opposition and cause the morale of the enemy to deteriorate unless his adversaries
are aware of the dictator's real aim. From then on he can start to build up his system
of conformity.
In both the Mindszenty and the Schwable cases, we have documented reports of the
techniques of menticide as it has been used to break the minds and wills of
courageous men.
Let us look first at the case of Cardinal Mindszenty, accused of misleading the
Hungarian people and collaboration with the enemies, the United States. In his
expose' on Cardinal Mindszenty imprisonment, Stephen K. Swift graphically
describes three typical phases in the psychological "processing" of political
prisoners. The first phase is directed toward extorting confession. The victim is
bombarded with questions day and night. He is inadequately and irregularly fed. He
is allowed almost no rest and remains in the interrogation chamber for hours on end
while his inquisitors take turns with him. Hungry, exhausted, his eyes blurred and
aching under unshaded lamps, the prisoner becomes little more than a hounded
animal.
"...when the Cardinal had been standing for sixty six hours [Swift reports], he closed
his eyes and remained silent. He did not even reply to questions with denials. The
colonel in charge of the shift tapped the Cardinal's shoulder and asked why he did
not respond. The Cardinal answered: 'End it all. Kill me! I am ready to die!' He was
told that no harm would come to him; that he could end it all simply by answering
certain questions.
"...By Saturday forenoon he could hardly be recognized. He asked for another drink
and this time it was refused. His feet and legs had swollen to such proportions that
they caused him intense pain; he fell down several times."
To the horrors the accused victim suffers from without must be added the horrors
from within. He is pursued by the unsteadiness of his own mind, which cannot
always produce the same answer to a repeated question. As a human being with a
conscience he is pursued by possible hidden guilt feelings, however pious he may
have been that undermine his rational awareness of innocence. The panic of the
"brainwashee" is the total confusion he suffers about all concepts. His evaluations
and norms are undermined. He cannot believe in anything objective any more except
in the dictated and indoctrinated logic of those who are more powerful than he. The
enemy knows that, far below the surface, human life is built up of inner
contradictions. He uses this knowledge to defeat and confuse the brainwashee. The
continual shift of interrogators makes it ever more impossible to believe in
consecutive thinking. Hardly has the victim adjusted himself to one inquisitor when
he has to change his focus of alertness to another one.
Yet, this inner clash of norms and concepts, this inner contradiction of ideologies and
beliefs is part of the philosophical sickness of our time!
As a social being the Cardinal is pursued by the need for good human relationships
and companionship. The constantly reiterated suggestion of his guilt urges him
toward confession. As a suffering individual he is blackmailed by an inner need to be
left alone and undisturbed, if only for a few minutes. From within and without he is
inexorably driven toward signing the confession prepared by his persecutors. Why should he resist any longer.
There are no visible witnesses to his heroism. He cannot prove his moral courage
and rectitude after his death. The core of the strategy of menticide is the taking away
of all hope, all anticipation, all belief in a future. It destroys the very elements which
keep the mind alive. The victim is utterly alone.
(NOTE: This continual attack on human conscience and guilt by unconscious self
accusations is brilliantly depicted by Franz Kafka in The Trial. In this novel the victim
never knows of what he is accused but his inner guilt leads him to conviction. Kafka
anticipated the age of blackmailing into confession. His novel was written before the
1930s. The same theme has been treated from a psychological point of view by
Theodor Reik in his Confession, Compulsion and the Need for Punishment.)
If the prisoner's mind proves too resistant, narcotics are given to confuse it:
mescaline, marijuana, morphine, barbiturates, alcohol. If his body collapses before
his mind capitulates, he receives stimulants: benzedrine, caffeine, coramine, all of
which help to preserve his consciousness until he confesses. Many of the narcotics
and stimuli which ultimately help to induce mental dependency and enforced
confusion can also create an amnesia, often a complete forgetting of the torture
itself. The torture techniques achieve the desired effect, but the victim forgets what
has actually happened during the interrogation. The clinicians who do therapeutic
work with amphetamine derivatives, which when injected into the blood stream help
patients to remember long forgotten experiences, are familiar with the drug's ability
to bring soothing forgetfulness of the period during which the patient was drugged
and questioned.
Next the victim is trained to accept his own confession, much as an animal is trained
to perform tricks. False admissions are reread, repeated, hammered into his brain.
He is forced to reproduce in his memory again and again the fantasized offenses,
fictitious details which ultimately convince him of his criminality. In the first stage he
is forced into mental submissiveness by others. In the second stage he has entered
a state of autohypnosis, convincing himself of fabricated crimes. According to Swift:
"The questions during the interrogation now dealt with details of the Cardinal's
'confession.' First his own statements were read to him; then statements of other
prisoners accused of complicity with him; then elaborations of those statements.
Sometimes the Cardinal was morose, sometimes greatly disturbed and excited. But
he answered all questions willingly, repeated all sentences once, twice, or even
three times when he was told to do so." (Lassio)
In the third and final phase of interrogation and menticide the accused, now
completely conditioned and accepting his own imposed guilt, is trained to bear false
witness against himself and others. He doesn't have to convince himself any more
through autohypnosis; he only speaks "his master's voice." He is prepared for trial,
softened completely; he becomes remorseful and willing to be sentenced. He is a
baby in the hands of his inquisitors, fed as a baby and soothed by words as a baby.
(NOTE: A more extended survey of the different psychological stages in menticide
and brainwashing will be given at the end of Chapter Four Why Do They Yield - The
Psychodynamics of False Confession.) 17s
Menticide in Korea
Now let us take a look at the Schwable case. In its general outline it is similar to the
Mindszenty story; it differs only in details. As an officer of the United States Marine
Corps, fighting with the United Nations in Korea, he is taken prisoner by the enemy.
The colonel expects to be protected by international law and by the regulations
regarding officer prisoners of war, which have been accepted by all countries.
However, it slowly dawns on him that he is being subjected to a kind of treatment
very different from what he expected. The enemy looks on him not as a prisoner of
war, but as a victim who can be used for propaganda purposes.
He is subjected to slow but constant pressures devised to break him down mentally.
Humiliation, rough, inhuman treatment, degradation, intimidation, hunger, exposure
to extreme cold all have been used to crumble his will and to soften him. They need
to wangle military secrets out of him and to use him as a tool in their propaganda
machine. He feels completely alone. He is surrounded by filth and vermin. For hours
on end he has to stand up and answer the questions his interrogators hurl at him. He
develops arthritic backache and diarrhoea. He is not allowed to wash or shave. He
doesn't know what will happen to him next. This treatment goes on for weeks.
Then the hours of systematic and repetitious interrogation and oppression increase.
He no longer dares to trust his own memory. There are new teams of investigators
every day, and each new team points out his increasing errors and mistakes. He
cannot sleep any more. Daily his interrogators tell him they have plenty of time, and
he realizes that in this respect at least they are telling the truth. He beings to doubt
whether he can resist their seductive propositions. If he will just unburden himself of
his guilt, they tell him, he will be better treated.
The inquisitor is treacherously kind and knows exactly what he wants. He wants the
victim captured by the influence of a slowly induced hypnosis. He wants a well
documented confession that the American army used bacteriological warfare, that
the captive himself took part in such germ warfare. The inquisitor wants this
confession in writing because it will make a convincing impression and will shock the
world. China is plagued by hunger and epidemics; such a confession will explain the
high disease rate and exculpate the Chinese government, whose popularity is at a
low ebb. So the colonel has to be prepared for a systematic confession, made before
an international group of Communist experts. Mentally and physically he is
weakened, and every day the Communist "truths" are imprinted on his mind.
The colonel has in fact become hypnotized; he is now able to reproduce for his
jailers bits and pieces of the confession they want from him. It is a well known
scientific fact that the passive memory often remembers facts learned under
hypnosis better than those learned in a state of alert consciousness. He is even able
to write some of it down. Eventually, all the little pieces fit, like a jigsaw puzzle, into a
complete, well organized whole; they form part of a document which was in fact
prepared beforehand by his captors. This document is placed in the colonel's hands,
and he is even allowed to make some minor changes in the phrasing before he signs
it.
By now, the colonel has been completely broken. He has given in. All sense of reality
is gone; identification with the enemy is complete. For weeks after signing the
confession he is in a state of depression. His only wish is to sleep, to have rest from
it all.
A man will often try to hold out beyond the limits of his endurance because he
continues to believe that his tormentors have some basic morality, that they will
finally realize the enormity of their crimes and will leave him alone. This is a delusion.
The only way to strengthen one's defences against an organized attack on the mind
and will is to understand better what the enemy is trying to do and to outwit him. Of
course, one can vow to hold out until death, but even the relief of death is in the
hands of the inquisitor. People can be brought to the threshold of death and then be
stimulated into life again so that the torments can be renewed. Attempts at suicide
are foreseen and can be forestalled.
In my opinion hardly anyone can resist such treatment. It all depends on the ego
strength of the person and the exhaustive technique of the inquisitor. Each man has
his own limit of endurance, but that this limit can nearly always be reached and even
surpassed is supported by clinical evidence. Nobody can predict for himself how he
will handle a situation when he is called to the test. The official United States report
on brainwashing (See the "New York Times", August 18, 1955) admits that "virtually
all American P.O.W.s collaborated at one time or another in one degree or another,
lost their identity as Americans...thousands lost their will to live," and so forth. The
British report (See the "New York Times", February 27, 1955) gives a statistical
survey about the abuse of the P.O.W.s. According to this report one third of the
soldiers absorbed enough indoctrination to be classified as Communist
sympathizers.
This same report describes in a more extended way some of the sadistic means
used by the enemy:
"If a prisoner accepted Communist doctrines, his life became easier, according to the
men's stories. But if a prisoner resisted Communist doctrines, the Chinese
considered him a criminal and reactionary deserving of any brutalities. the tortures
applied to the 'reactionaries' included:
"Making a prisoner stand at attention or sit with legs outstretched in complete silence
from 4:30 a.m. to 11:00 p.m. and constantly waking him during the few hours allowed
for sleep.
"Keeping prisoners in solitary confinement in boxes about five by three by two feet. A
private of the Gloucester Regiment spent more than six months in one of these.
"Withholding liquids for days 'to help self reflection'.
"Binding a prisoner with a rope passed over a beam, one end fixed as a hangman's
noose round his neck and the other end tied to his ankles. He was then told that if he
slipped or bent his knees he would be committing suicide.
"Forcing a prisoner to kneel on jagged rocks and hold a large rock over his head with
arms extended. It took a man who had undergone this treatment days to recover the
ability to walk.
"At one camp North Korean jailers pushed a pencil like piece of wood or metal
through a hole in the cell door and made the prisoner hold the inner end in his teeth.
Without warning a sentry would knock the outer end sidewise, breaking the man's
teeth or splitting the sides of his mouth. Sometimes the rod was rammed inward
against the back of the mouth or down the throat.
"Prisoners were marched barefooted to the frozen Yalu River, water was poured
over their feet and they were kept for hours with their feet frozen to the ice to 'reflect'
on their 'crimes.'"
Time, fear, and continual pressure are known to create a menticidal hypnosis. The
conscious part of the personality no longer takes part in the automatic confessions.
The brainwashed lives in a trance, repeating the record grooved into him and by
somebody else. Fortunately, this, too, is known: as soon as the victim returns to
normal circumstances, the panicky and hypnotic spell evaporates, and he again
awakens into reality.
This is what happened to Colonel Schwable. True, he confessed to crimes he did not
commit, but he repudiated his confession as soon as he was returned to a familiar
environment.
When, during the military inquiry into the Schwable case, I was called upon to testify
as an expert on menticide, I told the court of my deep conviction that nearly anybody
subjected to the treatment meted out to Colonel Schwable could be forced to write
and sign a similar confession.
"Anyone in this room, for instance?" the colonel's attorney asked me, looking in turn
at each of the officers sitting in judgment on this new and difficult case.
And in good conscience I could reply, firmly: "Anyone in this room."
It is now technically possible to bring the human mind into a condition of
enslavement and submission. The Schwable case and the cases of other prisoners
of war are tragic examples of this, made even more tragic by our lack of
understanding of the limits of heroism. We are just beginning to understand what
these limits are, and how they are used, both politically and psychologically, by the
totalitarians. We have long since come to recognize the breast beating confession
and the public recantation as propaganda tricks; now we are beginning to see ever
more clearly how the totalitarians use menticide: deliberately, openly, unashamedly,
as part of their official policy, as a means of consolidating and maintaining their
power, though, of course, they give a different explanation to the whole procedure it's
all confessions of real and treacherous crimes.
This brutal totalitarian technique has at least one virtue, however. It is obvious and
unmistakable, and we are learning to be on our guard against it, but as we shall see
later, there are other subtler forms of mental intervention. They can be just as
dangerous as the direct assault, precisely because they are more subtle and hence
more difficult to detect. Often we are not aware of their action at all. They influence
the mind so slowly and indirectly that we may not even realize what they have done
to us.
Like totalitarian menticide, some of these less obvious forms of mental manipulation
are political in purpose. Others are not. Even if they differ in intent, they can have the
same consequences.
These subtle menticidal forces operate both within the mind and outside it. They
have been strengthened in their effect by the growth in complexity of our civilization.
The modern means of mass communication bring the entire world daily into each
man's home; the techniques of propaganda and salesmanship have been refined
and systematized; there is scarcely any hiding place from the constant visual and
verbal assault on the mind. The pressures of daily life impel more and more people
to seek an easy escape from responsibility and maturity. Indeed, it is difficult to
withstand these pressures; to many the offer of a political panacea is very tempting,
to others the offer of escape through alcohol, drugs, or other artificial pleasures is
irresistible.
Free men in a free society must learn not only to recognize this stealthy attack on
mental integrity and fight it, but must learn also what there is in side man's mind that
makes him vulnerable to this attack, what it is that makes him, in many cases,
actually long for a way out of the responsibilities that republican democracy and
maturity place on him.
CHAPTER TWO
PAVLOV'S STUDENTS AS CIRCUS TAMERS
Before asking ourselves what the deeper mental mechanisms are of brainwashing,
false confession, and conversion into a collaborator, let us try to see things from the
standpoint of the totalitarian potentates. What is their aim? What terms do they use
to describe the behaviour of their prisoners? What do they want from the Schwables
and the Midszentys?
The totalitarian jailers don't speak of hypnosis or suggestion; they even deny the fact
of imposed confession. They think about human behaviour and human government
in a much more mechanical way. In order to understand them we have to give more
attention to their adoration of simplified Pavlovian concepts.
The Salivating Dog
In the latter part of the nineteenth century the Russian Nobel price winner Ivan
Petrovich Pavlov conducted his famous experiments with a bell and a dog. He knew
that salivation is associated with eating, and that if a dog was hungry, its mouth
would water each time it saw food. Pavlov took advantage of this useful inborn
reflex, which serves the digestive process, to develop in his experimental animal the
salivating response in answer to a stimulus which would not ordinarily create it. Each
time Pavlov fed the dog, he rang a bell, and at each feeding the dog's mouth
watered. Then after many repetitions of the combined food bell stimulus, Pavlov rang
the bell but did not feed the dog. The animal reacted to the bell alone just as it had
previously reacted to the sight of food its mouth watered. Thus the scientist had
found out that the dog could be induced to salivate involuntarily in response to an
arbitrary signal. It had been "conditioned" to respond to the ringing of the bell as if
that sound were the smell and taste of food.
From this and other experiments, Pavlov developed his theory of the conditioned
reflex, which explains learning and training as the building up of a mosaic of
conditioned reflexes, each one based on the establishment of an association
between different stimuli. The greater the number of learned complex responses also
called patterns the greater the number of conditioned reflexes developed. Because
man, of all the animals, has the greatest capacity for learning, he is the animal with
the greatest capacity for such complicated conditioning.
Pavlov's experiments were of great value in the study of animal and human
behaviour, and in the study of the development of neurotic symptoms. However, this
knowledge of some of the mechanisms of the human mind can be used as we have
seen already, like any other knowledge, either for good or for evil. And unfortunately,
the totalitarians have used their knowledge of how the mind works for their own
purposes
They have applied some of the Pavlovian findings, in a subtle and complicated way
and sometimes in a grotesque way, to try to produce the reflex of mental and political
conditioning and of submission in the human guinea pigs under their control.
Even though the Nazis employed these methods before the Second World War, they
can be said to have reached their full flower in Soviet Russia. Through a continued
repetition of indoctrination, bell ringing and feeding, the Soviet man is expected to
become a conditioned reflex machine, reacting according to a prearranged pattern,
as did the laboratory dogs. At least, such a simplified concept is roaming around in
the minds of some of the Soviet leaders and scientists (Dobrogaev).
In accordance with one of Stalin's directives, Moscow maintains a special "Pavlovian
Front" (Dobrogaev) and a "Scientific Council on Problems of Physiological Theory of
the Academician I. P. Pavlov" (London). These institutions, part of the Academy of
Science, are dedicated to the political application of the Pavlovian theory. They are
under orders to emphasize the purely mechanical aspects of Pavlov's findings. Such
a theoretical view can reduce all human emotions to a simple, mechanistic system of
conditioned reflexes. Both organizations are control agencies dealing in research
problems, and the scientists who work on them explore the ways in which man can
theoretically be conditioned and trained as animals are. Since Pavlovian theory is
proclaimed by the obdurate totalitarian theoreticians as the gospel of animal and
human behaviour, we have to grapple with the facts they adduce to prove their point,
and with their methods and theoretical explanations.
What the Pavlovian council tries to achieve is the result of an oversimplification of
psychology. Their political task is to condition and mould man's mind so that its
comprehension is confined to a narrow totalitarian concept of the world. It is the idea
that such a limitation of thinking to Lenin Marxist theoretical thinking must be
possible for two reasons: first, if one repeats often enough its simplification, and
second, if one withholds any other form of interpretation of reality.
This concept is based on the naive belief that one can permanently suppress any
critical function and verification in human thinking. Yet, through taming and
conditioning of people, during which period errors and deviations must continually be
corrected, unwittingly a critical sense is built up. True, at the same time the danger of
using this critical sense is brought home to the students. They know the dangers of
any dissent, but even this promotes the development of a secondary and more
refined critical sense. In the end, human rebellion and dissent cannot be suppressed;
they await only one breath of freedom in order to awake once more. The idea that
there exist other ways to truth than those he sees close at hand lives somewhere in
everybody. One can narrow his pathways of research and expression, but a man's
belief in adventurous new roads elsewhere is ever present in the back of his mind.
The inquisitive human mind is never satisfied with a simple recital of facts. As soon
as it observes a set of data, it jumps into the area of theory and offers explanations,
but the way a man sees a set of facts, and the way he juggles them to build them
into a theory is largely determined by his own biases and prejudices.
Let me be the first to confess that I am affected by my own subjectivities. Even the
words we use are loaded with implications and suggestions. The word "reflex," for
example, so important in Pavlovian theory, is a perfect instance of this. It was first
used by the seventeenth century philosopher Descartes, in whose philosophical
system a parallel was made between the actions of the human body and those of a
machine. For example, in the Cartesian view, the automatic reaction of the body to
certain painful stimuli (e.g., withdrawing the hand after it has come into contact with
fire) is compared with the automatic physical reflection of light from a mirror. The
nervous system, according to Descartes, reflects its response just as the mirror
does. Such a simple explanation of behaviour, and the very words used to describe
it, immediately denies the whole organism taking part in that response.
Yet man is not only a mirror, but a thinking mirror. According to the old mechanical
view, actions are associated only with the part of the body which performs them, and
they have no relationship whatsoever to the purposeful behaviour of the organism as
a whole. But man is not a machine composed of independently functioning parts. He
is a whole. His mind and body interact; he acts on the outside world and the outside
world acts on him. The innate reflexes, of which this hand withdrawal is one
example, are part of a whole system of adaptive responses which serve to help the
individual, as an entity, to adjust to changed circumstances. They can be described
as the result of an inborn adaptation tendency. The only real difference between the
innate reflexes and the conditioned reflexes is that the former supposedly have
developed in the entire race over the millions of years of the evolutionary process,
while the latter are developed during the life span of the individual as a result of the
gradual automatization of acquired responses.
If you analyze any one of the complicated actions you may perform during the
course of a single day (driving an automobile, for example), you will see that it
occurs outside your conscious management. And yet, before the process could be
automatized, the actions, purposefully directed toward the satisfaction of some goal,
had to be consciously learned and managed. You were not born with the innate
reflex of jamming on the brake to stop a car quickly in an emergency. You had to
learn to do it, and in the process of learning and driving, this response became
automatic. If, after you have learned to drive, you see a child running across the path
of your car, you put the brake on immediately, by reflex, without thinking.
The Conditioning of Man
Pavlov's research on the machinery of the mind taught us how all the animals
including man learn adjustment to existing limitations through linking the signs and
signals of life to body reactions. The mind creates a relationship between repeated
simultaneous occurrences, and the body reacts to the connections the mind forms.
Thus the bell, rung each time the dog was fed, became a signal to the animal to
prepare for digestion, and the animal began to salivate.
Recent experiments conducted by Dr. Gregory Razran of Queens College show how
men may develop these same kinds of responses. Dr. Razran treated a group of
twenty college students to a series of free luncheons at which music was played or
pictures shown. After the final luncheon, these twenty students were brought
together with another group who had not been luncheon guests. At this meeting, as
at the luncheons, music was played and pictures shown, and all the students were
asked to tell what the music and pictures made them think of. The music and the
pictures generally reminded the first group of something related to eating, but had no
such associations for the second group. There was obviously a temporary
connection in the minds of the luncheon guests between the music and the pictures
on the one hand and eating on the other.
The Chinese did their mass conditioning in an even simpler way. After having taught
the prisoners for days to write down all possible nonsense and political lies in an
atmosphere of utter confusion and stress they were ripe to sign collectively the lie of
having taken part in germ warfare (Winokur.)
All conditioned reflexes are involuntary temporary adjustments to pressures which
create an apparent connection between stimuli which may be in fact totally
unrelated. For this reason, the conditioned reflex is not necessarily permanently
imprinted on the individual, but can gradually disappear. If, after the dog's
conditioned reflex to the bell has been developed, the bell is rung over and over
again and no food is presented to the animal, the salivating reflex disappears.
Doubtless Dr. Razran's students will not always think of food when they hear music.
We could describe the conditioned reflex another way: it is a selected response of
the mind body unit to a given stimulus. The ways in which the stimulus and the
response are connected vary considerably they may have been associated in time,
in place, or by coincidence, or by a common aim and thus they may form a special
conditioned complex in our mental and physical attitude. Some of these complex
responses or patterns are more autonomous than others, and will act like the innate
patterns. Some are flexible and are continually changing. Analysis of some of the
psychosomatic diseases, for example, shows us how our inner emotional attitudes
can intensify or even change a conditional response. Stomach ulcers are considered
an example of such a psychosomatic disease. The mother who puts her child on a
too rigid feeding schedule may change the child's favourable response to hunger into
a stubborn reaction against feeding.
For our purpose we have to be aware that conditioning takes place throughout all our
lives in the most subtle and in the most obvious ways. We discover that the moulding
of our personalities may occur in a thousand fold ways through such matters as
these: the meal training given in early childhood; the harshness or the musical tone
of the words spoken to us; the sense of haste in our surroundings; the steadiness of
family habits or the chaos of neurotic parents; the noises of our machines; the
reservedness of our friends; the discipline of our schools and the competitiveness of
our clubs. We are even conditioned by such things as the frailty of our toys and the
cosiness of our houses, the steadiness of traditions or the chaos of a revolution. The
artist and the engineer, the teacher and the friend, the uncle or aunt they all give
shape to our behaviour.
Isolation and Other Factors in Conditioning
Pavlov made another significant discovery: the conditioned reflex could be
developed most easily in a quiet laboratory with a minimum of disturbing stimuli.
Every trainer of animals knows this from his own experience; isolation and the
patient repetition of stimuli are required to tame wild animals. Pavlov formulated his
findings into a general rule in which the speed of learning is positively correlated with
quiet and isolation. The totalitarians have followed this rule. They know that they can
condition their political victims most quickly if they are kept in isolation. In the
totalitarian technique of thought control, the same isolation applied to the individual is
applied also to groups of people. This is the reason the civilian populations of the
totalitarian countries are not permitted to travel freely and are kept away from mental
and political contamination. It is the reason, to, for the solitary confinement cell and
the prison camp.
Another of Pavlov's findings was that some animals learn more quickly if they were
rewarded (by affection, by food, by stroking) each time they showed the right
response, while others learned more quickly when the penalty for not learning was a
painful stimulus. In human terms, the latter animals could be described as learning in
order to avoid punishment. These different reactions in animals may perhaps be
related to an earlier conditioning by the parents, and they find their counterparts
among human beings. In some people the strategy of reward and flattery is a
stimulus to learning, while pain evokes all their resistance and rebellion; in others
retribution and punishment for failure can be a means of training them into the
desired pattern. Before he can do his job effectively, the brainwasher has to find out
to which category his victim belongs. There are people more amenable to
brainwashing than others. Part of the response may be innate or related to earlier
conditioning to conformity.
Pavlov also distinguished between the weaker type of involuntary learning, in which
the learned response was lost as soon as some disturbance occurred, and the
stronger type, in which training was retained through all kinds of changed conditions.
As a matter of fact, Pavlov described more types of learning than this, but for our
purposes it is only important to know that there are some types of people who lose
their conditioned learning easily, while others, the so called "stronger" types, retain it.
This, by the way, is another example of how our choice of words reflects our bias.
The descriptions "strong" and "weak" depend completely on the aim of the
experimenter. For the totalitarian, the "weak" P.O.W. is the man who stubbornly
refuses to accept the new conditioning. His "weakness" may be, in fact, a resistance,
the result of a previous strong conditioning to loyalty to anti totalitarian principles. We
never know how strongly conditioning and initial learning are impressed on the
personality. Rigid dogmatic behaviour has its roots in early conditioning and so may
submissiveness based on ignorance rather than knowledge.
Pavlov showed, too, how internal and external factors interact in the conditioning
process. If, for example, a new laboratory assistant was brought in to work with the
animals, all of their newly acquired patterns could easily be inhibited because of the
animals' emotional reactions to the newcomer. Pavlov explained this as a disruptive
reaction caused by the animals' investigatory reflexes, which led them to sniff around
the stranger. Current psychology tends to interpret it as the result of the changed
emotional rapport between the animal and its trainers. We can easily expand the
implications of this more modern view into the field of human relations. It points up
the fact that there are some persons who can create such immediate rapport with
others that the latter will soon give up many old habits and ways of life to conform
with new demands. There are inquisitors and investigators whose personalities so
deeply affect their victims that the victims speedily yield their secrets and accept
entirely new ways of thinking.
We can see the same thing in psychotherapy, where the development of an
emotional rapport between doctor and patient is the most important factor leading to
cure. In some cases rapport can be established immediately, in others rapport
cannot be built up at all, in most cases it develops gradually during the course of the
therapy. It is not difficult for a psychologist to test a man's "softness" and willingness
to be conditioned, and as a matter of fact the Pavlovians have developed simple
questionnaires through which they can easily determine a given individual's
instability and adaptability to suggestion and brainwashing.
Pavlov found that all conditioning, no matter how strong it had been, became
inhibited through boredom or through the repetition of too weak signals. The bell
could no longer arouse salivation in the experimental dogs if it was repeated too
often or its tone was too soft. A process of unlearning took place. The result of such
internal inhibition of conditioning and the loss of conditioned reflex action is sleep.
The inhibition spreads over the entire activity of the brain cortex; the organism falls
into a hypnotic state. This explanation of the process of inhibition was one of the first
acceptable theories of sleep. An interesting psychological question is whether too
much official conditioning causes boredom and inhibition, and whether that is the
reason why the Stakhanovite movement in Russia was necessary to counteract the
loss of productivity of the people.
We can make a comparison with what happened to our prisoners of war in Korea.
Under the daily signal of dulling routine questions for every word can act as a
Pavlovian signal their minds went into a state of inhibition and diminished alertness.
This made it possible for them to give up temporarily their former democratic
conditioning and training. When they had unlearned and suppressed the democratic
way, their inquisitors could start teaching them the totalitarian philosophy. First the
old patterns have to be broken down in order to build up new conditioned reflexes.
We can imagine that boredom and repetition arouse the need to give in and to yield
to the provoking words of the enemy. Later I shall come back to the system of
negative stimuli used in conditioning for brainwashing.
Mass Conditioning Through Speech
According to official Pavlovian psychology, human speech is also a conditioned
reflex activity. Pavlov distinguished between stimuli of the first order, which condition
men and animals directly, and stimuli of the second order, with weaker and more
complicated conditioning qualities. In this so called second signal system, verbal
cues replace the original physical sound stimuli. Pavlov himself did not give much
attention to this second signal system. It was especially after Stalin's publication in
1950 on the significance of linguistics for mass indoctrination (as quoted by
Dobrogaev) that the Russian psychologists began to do work in this area. In his
letter, Stalin followed Engel's theory that language is the characteristic human bit of
adaptive equipment. That tone and sound in speech have a conditioning quality is
something we can verify from our own experience in listening to or in giving
commands, or in dealing with our pets. Even the symbolic and semantic meaning of
words can acquire a conditioning quality. The word "traitor," for example, provokes
direct feelings and reactions in the minds of those who hear it spoken, even if this
discriminatory label is being applied dishonestly.
Through an elaborate study on speech reflexes written by one of the leading Russian
psychologists, Dobrogaev, we get a fairly good insight into the ways in which speech
patterns and word signals are used in the service of mass conditioning, by means of
propaganda and indoctrination. The basic problems for the man tamer are rather
simple: Can man resist a government bent on conditioning him? What can the
individual do to protect his mental integrity against the power of a forceful
collectivity? Is it possible to do away with every vestige of inner resistance?
Pavlov had already explained that man's relation to the external world, and to his
fellow men, is dominated by secondary stimuli, the speech symbols. Man learns to
think in words and in the speech figures given him, and these gradually condition his
entire outlook on life and on the world. As Dobrogaev says, "Language is the means
of man's adaptation to his environment." We could rephrase that statement in this
way: man's need for communication with his fellow men interferes with his relation to
the outside world, because language and speech itself the verbal tools we use are
variable and not objective. Dobrogaev continues: "Speech manifestations represent
conditioned reflex functions of the human brain." In a simpler way we may say: he
who dictates and formulates the words and phrases we use, he who is master of the
press and radio, is master of the mind.
In the Pavlovian strategy, terrorizing force can finally be replaced by a new
organization of the means of communication. Ready made opinions can be
distributed day by day through press, radio, and so on, again and again, till they
reach the nerve cell and implant a fixed pattern of thought in the brain.
Consequently, guided public opinion is the result, according to Pavlovian
theoreticians, of good propaganda technique, and the polls a verification of the
temporary successful action of the Pavlovian machinations on the mind. Yet, the
polls may only count what people pretend to think and believe, because it is
dangerous for them to do otherwise.
Such is the Pavlovian device: repeat mechanically your assumptions and
suggestions diminish the opportunity of communicating dissent and opposition. This
is the simple formula for political conditioning of the masses. This is also the actual
ideal of some of our public relation machines, who thus hope to manipulate the
public into buying a special soap or voting for a special party.
The Pavlovian strategy in public relations has people conditioned more and more to
ask themselves, "What do other people think?" As a result, a common delusion is
created: people are incited to think what other people think, and thus public opinion
may mushroom out into a mass prejudice.
Expressed in psychoanalytic terms, through daily propagandistic noise backed up by
forceful verbal cues, people can more and more be forced to identify with the
powerful noisemaker. Big Brother's voice resounds in all the little brothers. [ you mean like the current nonsense of 'white supremacy'? dc]
News from Red China, as reported by neutral Indian journalists (See the "New York
Times", November 27, 1954) tells us that the Chinese leaders are using this vocal
conditioning of the public to strengthen their regime. Throughout the country, radios
and loud speakers are broadcasting the official "truths." The sugary voices take
possession of people, the cultural tyranny traps their ears with loud speakers, telling
them what they may and may not do. This microphone regimentation was foreseen
by the French philosopher La Rochefoucauld, who, in the eighteenth century, said:
"A man is like a rabbit, you catch him by the ears."
During the Second World War the Nazis showed that they too were very much aware
of this conditioning power of the word. I saw their strategy at work in Holland. The
radio constantly spread political suggestions and propaganda, and people were
obliged to listen because the simple act of turning off one's radio was in itself
suspicious. I remember one day during the occupation when I was taking a bicycle
trip with some friends. We stopped off to rest at a cafe that, we later realized, was a
true Nazi nest. When the radio, which had been on ever since we arrived,
announced a speech by Hitler, everyone stood up in awe, and it was a must to take
in the verbal conditioning by the Fuhrer. My friends and I had to stand up too, and
were forced to listen to that raucous voice crackling in our ears and to summon all
our resistance against that long, boring, repetitive attack on our eardrums and minds.
Throughout the occupation, the Nazis printed tons of propaganda, Big Lies, and
distortions. They even went so far as to paint their slogans on the stoops of the
houses and in the streets. Every week newly fabricated stereotypes ogled at us as if
to convince us of the splendour of the Third Reich. But the Nazis did not know the
correct Pavlovian strategy. By satisfying their own need to discuss and to vary their
arguments in order to make them seem more logical, they only increased the
resistance of the Dutch people. This resistance was additionally fortified by the
London radio, on which the Dutch could hear the sane voice of their own legal
government. Had the Nazis not argued and justified so much, and had they been
able to prevent all written, printed, or spoken communication, the long period of
boredom would have inhibited our democratic conditioning, and we might well have
been more seduced by the Nazi oversimplifications and slogans.
Political Conditioning 30s
Political conditioning should not be confused with training or persuasion or even
indoctrination. It is more than that. It is tampering. It is taking possession of both the
simplest and the most complicated nervous patterns of man. It is the battle for the
possession of the nerve cells. It is coercion and enforced conversion. Instead of
conditioning man to an unbiased facing of reality, the seducer conditions him to
catchwords, verbal stereotypes, slogans, formulas, symbols. Pavlovian strategy in
the totalitarian sense means imprinting prescribed reflexes on a mind that has been
broken down. The totalitarian wants first the required response from the nerve cells,
then control of the individual, and finally control of the masses. The system starts
with verbal conditioning and training by combining the required stereotypes with
negative or positive stimuli: pain, or reward. In the P.O.W. camps in Korea where
there was individual and mass brainwashing, the negative and positive conditioning
stimuli were usually hunger and food. The moment the soldier conformed to the party
line his food ration was improved: say yes, and I'll give you a piece of candy!
The whole gamut of negative stimuli, as we saw them in the Schwable case, consists
of physical pressure, moral pressure, fatigue, hunger, boring repetition, confusion by
seemingly logical syllogisms. Many victims of totalitarianism have told me in
interviews that the most upsetting experience they faced in the concentration camps
was the feeling of loss of logic, the state of confusion into which they had been
brought the state in which nothing had any validity. They had arrived at the Pavlovian
state of inhibition, which psychiatrists call mental disintegration or depersonalization.
It seemed as if they had unlearned all their former responses and had not yet
adopted new ones. But in reality they simply did not know what was what.
The Pavlovian theory translated into a political method, as a way of levelling the
mind (the Nazis called it "Gleichschaltung") is the stock in trade of totalitarian
countries. Some psychiatric points are of interest because we see that Pavlovian
training can be used successfully only when special mental conditions prevail. In
order to tame people into the desired pattern, victims must be brought to a point
where they have lost their alert consciousness and mental awareness. Freedom of
discussion and free intellectual exchange hinder conditioning. Feelings of terror,
feelings of fear and hopelessness, of being alone, of standing with one's back to the
wall, must be instilled.
The treatment of American prisoners of war in the Korean P.O.W. camps followed
just such a pattern. They were compelled to listen to lectures and other forms of daily
word barrage. The very fact that they did not understand the lectures and were bored
by the long sessions inhibited their democratic training, and conditioned them to
swallow passively the bitter doctrinal diet, for the prisoners were subjected not only
to a political training program, but also to an involuntary taming program. To some
degree the Communist propaganda lectures were directed toward retraining the
prisoners' minds. This training our soldiers could reject, but the endless repetitions
and the constant sloganizing, together with the physical hardships and deprivations
the prisoners suffered, caused an UNCONSCIOUS TAMING and conditioning,
against which only previously built up inner strength and awareness could help.
There is still another reason why our soldiers were sometimes trapped by the
Communist conditioning. Experiments with animals and experiences with human
beings have taught us that threat, tension, and anxiety, in general, may accelerate
the establishment of conditioned responses, particularly when those responses tend
to diminish fear and panic (Spence and Farber). The emergency of prison camp life
and mental torture provide ideal circumstances for such conditioning. The responses
can develop even when the victim is completely unaware that he is being influenced.
Thus, many of our soldiers developed automatic responses of which they remained
completely unconscious (Segal). But this is only one side of the coin, for experience
has also shown that people who know what to expect under conditions of mental
pressure can develop a so called perceptual defense, which protects them from
being influenced. This means that the more familiar people are with the concepts of
thought control and menticide, the more they understand the nature of the
propaganda barrage directed against them, the more inner resistance they can put
up, even though inevitably some of the inquisitor's suggestions will leak through the
barrier of conscious mental defense.
Our understanding of the conditioning process leads us also to an understanding of
some of the paradoxical reactions found among victims of concentration camps and
other prisoners. Often those with a rigid, simple belief were better able to withstand
the continual barrage against their minds than were the flexible, sophisticated ones,
full of doubt and inner conflicts. The simple man with deep rooted, freely absorbed
religious faith could exert a much greater inner resistance than could the complex,
questioning intellectualist. The refined intellectual is much more handicapped by the
internal pros and cons.
In totalitarian countries, where belief in Pavlovian strategy has assumed grotesque
proportions, the self thinking, subjective man has disappeared. There is an utter
rejection of any attempt at persuasion or discussion. Individual self expression is
taboo. Private affection is taboo.
Peaceful exchange of free thoughts in free conversation will disturb the conditioned
reflexes and is therefore taboo. No longer are there any brains, only conditioned
patterns and educated muscles. In such a taming system neurotic compulsion is
looked upon as a positive asset instead of something pathological. The mental
automaton becomes the ideal of education.
Yet the Soviet theoreticians themselves are often unaware of this, and many of them
do not realize the dire consequences of subjecting man to a completely mechanistic
conditioning. They themselves are often just as frightened as we are by the picture of
the perfectly functioning human robot. This is what one of their psychologists says:
"The entire reactionary nature of this approach to man is completely clear. Man is an
automaton who can be caused to act as one wills! This is the ideal of capitalism!
Behold the dream of capitalism the world over a working class without
consciousness, which cannot think for itself, whose actions can be trained according
to the whim of the exploiter! This is the reason why it is in America, the bulwark of
present day capitalism, that the theory of man as a robot has been so vigorously
developed and so stubbornly held to." (Bauer)
Western psychology and psychiatry, although acknowledging its debt to Pavlov as a
great pioneer who made important contributions to our understanding of behaviour,
takes a much less mechanical view of man than do the Soviet Pavlovians. It is
apparent to us that their simple explanation of training ignores and rejects the
concept of purposeful adaptation and the question of the goals to which this training
is directed. Western experimental psychologists tend to see the conditioned reflex as
developing fully only in the service of gratifying basic instinctual needs or of avoiding
pain, that is, only when the whole organism is concerned in the activity. In that
complicated process of response to the world, conscious, and especially
unconscious, drives and motivations play a role.
All training, of which the conditioned response is only one example, is an
automatization of actions which were originally consciously learned and thought
over. The ideal of Western democratic psychology is to train men into independence
and maturity by enlisting their conscious aid, awareness, and volition in the learning
process. The ideal of the totalitarian psychology, on the other hand, is to tame men,
to make them willing tools in the hands of their leaders. Like training, taming has the
purpose of making actions automatic; unlike training, it does not require the
conscious participation of the learner. Both training and taming are energy and
timesaving devices, and in both the mystery of the psyche is hidden in the
purposefulness of the responses. The automatization of functions in man saves him
expenditure of energy but can make him weaker when encountering new
unexpected challenges.
Cultural routinization and habit formation by local rules and myths make of
everybody a partial automaton. National and racial prejudices are acted out
unwittingly. Group hatred often bursts out almost automatically when triggered by
slogans and catchwords. In a totalitarian world, this narrow disciplinarian
conditioning is done more "perfectly" and more "ad absurdum."
The Urge to Be Conditioned
One suggestion this chapter is not intended to convey is that Pavlovian conditioning
as such is something wrong. This kind of conditioning occurs everywhere where
people are together in common interaction. The speaker influences the listener, but
the listener also the speaker. Through the process of conditioning people often learn
to like and to do what they are allowed to like and do. The more isolated the group,
the stricter the conditioning that takes place in those belonging to the group. In some
groups one finds people more capable than others of conveying suggestion and
bringing about conditioning. Gradually one can discern the stronger ones, the better
adjusted ones, the more experienced ones, and those noisier ones, whose ability to
condition others is strongest. Every group, every club, every society has its leading
Pavlovian Bell. This kind of person imprints his inner bell ringing on others. He can
even develop a system of monolithic bell ringing: no other influential bell is allowed to
compete with him.
Another subtler question belongs to these problems. Why is there in us so great an
urge to be conditioned, the urge to learn, to imitate, to conform, and to follow the
pattern of family and group? This urge to be conditioned, to submit to the communal
pattern and the family pattern must be related to man's dependency on parents and
fellow men. Animals are not so dependent on one another. In the whole animal
kingdom man is one of the most helpless and naked beings. But among the animals
man has, relatively, the longest youth and time for learning.
Puzzlement and doubt, which inevitably arise in the training process, are the
beginnings of mental freedom. Of course, the initial puzzlement and doubt is not
enough. Behind that there has to be faith in our democratic freedoms and the will to
fight for it. I hope to come back to this central problem of faith in moral freedom as
differentiated from conditioned loyalty and servitude in the last chapter. Puzzlement
and doubt are, however, already crimes in the totalitarian state. The mind that is
open for questions is open for dissent. In the totalitarian regime the doubting,
inquisitive, and imaginative mind has to be suppressed. The totalitarian slave is only
allowed to memorize, to salivate when the bell rings.
It is not my task here to elaborate on the subject of the biased use of Pavlovian rules
by totalitarians, but without doubt part of the interpretation of any psychology is
determined by the ways we think about our fellow human beings and man's place in
nature. If our ideal is to make conditioned zombies out of people, the current misuse
of Pavlovianism will serve our purpose. But once we become even vaguely aware
that in the totalitarian picture of man the characteristic human note is missing, and
when see that in such a scheme man sacrifices his instinctual desires, his pleasures,
his aims, his goals, his creativity, his instinct for freedom, his paradoxicality, we
immediately turn against this political perversion of science. Such use of Pavlovian
technique is aimed only at developing the automaton in man, not his free alert mind
that is aware of moral goals and aims in life.
Even in laboratory animals we have found that affective goal directedness can spoil
the Pavlovian experiment. When, during a bell food training session, the dog's
beloved master entered the room, the animal lost all its previous conditioning and
began to bark excitedly. Here is a simple example of an age old truth: love and
laughter break through all rigid conditioning. The rigid automaton cannot exist
without spontaneous self expression. Apparently, the fact that the dog's spontaneous
affection for his master could ruin all the mechanical calculations and manipulations
never occurred to Pavlov's totalitarian students.
next35
CHAPTER THREE
MEDICATION INTO SUBMISSION
No comments:
Post a Comment