The Body Electric
by Robert O. Becker
3
The Sign of the Miracle
When you want to do a research project, there are certain channels
you must go through to get the money. You write a project proposal,
spelling out what hypothesis you want to test, why you think it should
be done, and how you plan to go about it. The proposal goes to a
committee supposedly composed of your peers, people who have demonstrated competence in related research. If they approve your project and
the money is available, you generally get part of what you asked for,
enough to get started.
The Veterans Administration had been dispensing research money for
several years as a sort of bribe to attract doctors despite the low pay in
government service. The money from Washington was doled out by the
most influential doctors on the staff, not necessarily the best researchers,
but I still felt I had a good chance because the VA was having an especially hard time recruiting orthopedists. Moreover, my hypothesis was
based on the work of Rose, Polezhaev, Singer, Sinyukhin, and Zhirmunskii with inescapable logic. And since frogs and salamanders were
anatomically similar, any difference in their currents of injury should
reflect the disparity in their powers of regeneration. My chances of being
thrown off by extraneous factors were thus minimal.
I remember thinking, as I wrote the proposal, how my life had come
full circle. As a college freshman in 1941, I'd conducted a crude experiment on salamanders, showing that thyroid stimulation by iodine didn't
speed up regeneration. Here I was nearly twenty years later, beneficiary
of the intervening research, hoping to add to our knowledge of the same
phenomenon and perhaps even discover something that would help human patients. I worried that my roundabout course might weigh against
me, since one of the criteria for grants was whether the investigator had
been trained for that particular field. This proposal would have been
expected from a physiologist, not an orthopedist. Nevertheless, I was
asking for a relatively minuscule amount of money. I needed only a
thousand dollars to put together the equipment, so I didn't anticipate
much trouble.
The Tribunal
"Dr. Becker, could you please come to a special research committee
meeting in one hour?" The committee's secretary was calling. I'd known
something was up, two months had passed since I'd filed my proposal,
and all my queries as to its fate had gone unanswered.
"I'll be there."
"It's not here in the research office. It's downstairs in the hospital
director's office." Now that was really strange. The director almost never
paid any attention to the research program. Besides, his office was big
enough to hold a barbecue in.
It was a barbecue, all right, and I was the one being grilled. The
director's conference room had been rearranged. In place of the long,
polished table there was a semicircle of about a dozen chairs, each occupied by one of the luminaries from the hospital and medical school. I
recognized the chairmen of the departments of biochemistry and physiology along with the hospital director and chief of research. Only the
dean was missing. In the center was a single chair—for me.
The spokesman came right to the point: "We have a very grave basic
concern over your proposal. This notion that electricity has anything to
do with living things was totally discredited some time ago. It has absolutely no validity, and the new scientific evidence you're citing is worthless. The whole idea was based on its appeal to quacks and the gullible
public. I will not stand idly by and see this medical school associated
with such a charlatanistic, unscientific project." Murmurs of assent
spread around the group.
I had the momentary thrill of imagining myself as Galileo or Giordano Bruno; I thought of walking to the window to see if the stake and
fagots were set up on the lawn. Instead I delivered a terse speech to the
effect that I still thought my hypothesis was stoutly supported by some
very good research and that I was sorry if it flew in the face of dogma. I
ended by saying that I didn't intend to withdraw the proposal, so they
would have to act upon it.
When I got home, my fury was gone. I was ready to call the director,
withdraw my proposal, apologize for my errors, stay out of research,
quit the VA, and go into private practice, where I could make a lot
more money. Luckily, my wife Lil knows me better than I sometimes
know myself. She told me, "You'd be miserable in private practice. This
is exactly what you want to do, so just wait and see what happens."
Two days later I got word that the committee had delegated the decision to Professor Chester Yntema, an anatomist who long ago had studied the regrowth of ears in the salamander. Since he was the only one in
Syracuse who'd ever done any regeneration research, I've always wondered why he wasn't part of the first evaluation. I went to see him with a
sense of foreboding, for his latest research seemed to refute Singer's nerve
work, on which I'd based my proposal.
Using a standard technique, Yntema had operated on very young salamander embryos, cutting out all of the tissues that would have given rise to the nervous system. He then grafted each of these denervated
embryos onto the back of a whole one. The intact embryos furnished the
grafts with blood and nourishment, and the procedure resulted in a little
"parabiotic" twin, normal except for having no nerves, stuck on the
back of each host animal. Yntema then cut off one leg from each of these
twins, and some of them regenerated. Since microscopic examination
revealed no nerves entering the graft from its host, Yntema's experiment
called Singer's conclusions into question.
Dr. Yntema turned out to be one of the nicest gentlemen I've ever
met, but as I entered his office his Dickensian appearance of eminence—
he was tall, thin, elderly, with craggy features, and wore an immaculately starched, long white lab coat—made me feel like a freshman
being called before the dean. But he put me at ease immediately.
"I've read your proposal and think it's most intriguing," he said with
genuine interest.
"Do you really?" I asked. "I've been afraid you would reject it out of
hand because my ideas depend on Singer's work."
"Marc Singer is a good, careful worker," Yntema replied. "I don't
doubt his observations. What I've described is an exception to his findings under special circumstances."
After a long, pleasant conversation about regrowth, nerves, and research itself, he gave me his approval with a word of caution: "Don't get
your hopes up about what you want to do. I don't believe for one minute
that it'll work, but I think you should do it anyway. We need to encourage young researchers. Besides, it'll be fun, and maybe you'll learn
something new, after all. Let me know what happens, and if you need
any help, I'll be here. I'll call the people at the VA right away, so get to
work. Good luck."
This was the start of a long friendship. I'm deeply indebted to Chester
Yntema for his encouragement. Had he not believed that research should
be fun, that you should do what you want rather than what's fashionable, my first experiment would have been impossible, and this book
would never have been written.
The Reversals
First I found a good supplier of salamanders and frogs, a Tennessee game
warden who ran this business in his spare time. Sometimes the shipment
would contain a surprise, a small snake. I never found out whether he
included them deliberately or by error. At any rate, his animals weren't the inferior aquarium-bred stock but robust specimens collected from
their natural habitats.
Next I worked out some technical problems. The most important of
these was the question of where to place the electrodes. To form the
circuit, two electrodes had to touch the animal. One was the "hot" or
measuring electrode, which determined the polarity, positive or negative, with regard to a stationary reference electrode. A negative polarity
meant there were more electrons where the measuring electrode was
placed, while a positive polarity meant there were more at the reference
site. A steady preponderance of negative charge at a particular location
could mean there was a current flowing toward that spot, continually
replenishing the accumulation of electrons. The placement of the reference electrode, therefore, was critical, lest I get the voltage right but the
polarity, and hence the direction of the current, wrong. Some logical
position had to be chosen and used every time.
Since I postulated that
the nerves were somehow related to the current, the cell bodies that sent
their nerve fibers into the limb seemed like a good reference point.
These cell bodies were in a section of the spinal cord called the brachial
enlargement, located just headward from where the arm joined the
body. In both frogs and salamanders, therefore, I put the measuring
electrode directly on the cut surface of the amputation stump and the
reference electrode on the skin over the brachial enlargement.
After setting up the equipment, I did some preliminary measurements on the intact animals. They all had areas of positive charge at the
brachial enlargement and a negative charge of about 8 to 10 millivolts at
each extremity, suggesting a flow of electrons from the head and trunk
out into the limbs and, in the salamanders, the tail.
I began the actual experiment by amputating the right forelimbs,
between elbow and wrist, from fourteen salamanders and fourteen grass
frogs, all under anesthesia. I took no special precautions against bleeding, since blood clots formed very rapidly. The wounds had to be left
open, not only because closing the skin over the salamanders' amputation sites would have stopped regeneration, but also because I was investigating a natural process. In the wild, both frogs and salamanders
get injuries much like the one I was producing—both are favorite foods
of the freshwater bass—and heal them without a surgeon.
Once the anesthetic wore off and the blood clot formed, I took a
voltage reading from each stump. I was surprised to find that the polarity at the crump reversed to positive right after the injury. By the next
day it had climbed to over 20 millivolts, the same in both frogs and
salamanders.
I made measurements daily, expecting to see the salamander voltages
climb above those of the frogs as the blastemas formed. It didn't work
that way. The force of the current flowing from the salamanders' amputation sites rapidly dropped, while that from the frogs' stumps stayed
at the original level. By the third day the salamanders showed no current
at all, and their blastemas hadn't even begun to appear.
The experiment seemed a failure. I almost quit right there, but something made me keep on measuring. I guess I thought it would be good
practice.
Then, between the sixth and tenth days an exciting trend emerged.
The salamander potentials changed their sign again, exceeding their normal voltage and reaching a peak of more than 30 millivolts negative just
when the blastemas were emerging. The frogs were still plugging away
with slowly declining positive voltages. As the salamander limbs regenerated and the frog stumps healed over with skin and scar tissue, both
groups of limbs gradually returned (from opposite directions) to the
original baseline of 10 millivolts negative.
Here was confirmation better than my wildest dreams! Already, in my
first experiment, I had the best payoff research can give—the excitement
of seeing something no one else seen before. I knew now that
74 The Body Electric
the current of injury wasn't due to dying cells, which were long gone by
then. Moreover, the opposite polarities indicated a profound difference
in the electrical properties of the two animals, which somehow would
explain why only the salamander could regenerate. The negative potential seemed to bring forth the all-important blastema. It was a very
significant observation, even though the facts had scrambled my neat
Dr. Yntema agreed and urged me to write up a report for publication,
but first I jumped ahead with another idea. I took a new group of frogs,
amputated one foreleg from each, and every day applied negative current
to the stump from a small battery. I dreamed of being the first to get
complete regrowth in a normally non-regenerating animal; I could almost
see my name on the cover of Scientific American. The frogs were less
interested in my glory. They had to hold still for up to half an hour with
electrodes attached. They refused, so I anesthetized them each day,
something they tolerated very poorly. Within a week my Nobel Prize
had turned into a collection of dead frogs.
For some time I'd been scouring the dusty stacks of the medical library for previous work on bioelectricity, and how I found a paper written in 1909 by an American researcher named Owen E. Frazee. He
reported that electrical currents passed through the aquarium water in which larval salamanders were living speeded up their regeneration. At
that time, electrical equipment was so primitive that I couldn't rely on
Frazee's results, but I decided to try it for myself. What Sinyukhin had
done with tomato plants I hoped to do with salamanders.
To one group of salamanders I applied 2 microamperes of positive
current from batteries connected directly to the stumps for five to ten
minutes on each of the first five days after amputation. This was
0.000002 ampere, a tiny current by ordinary standards (most household
circuits carry 15 or 20 amperes) but comparable to what seemed to be
flowing in the limb. I intended to reinforce the normal positive peak in
the current of injury. This treatment seemed to make the blastemas
larger but slowed down the whole process somewhat. To another group I
applied 3 microamperes of negative current on the fifth to ninth days,
when the normal currents were hitting their negative peaks. This
seemed to increase the rate of regrowth for a week but didn't change the
time needed for a complete limb. Finally I tried Frazee's method with a
constant current through the aquarium water. Again the results were
equivocal at best. These failures taught me that, before I applied my
findings to other animals, I would have to learn how the current of injury
worked.
Meanwhile, I wrote up my results. Not knowing any better, I submitted my paper to the Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery, the most prestigious orthopedic journal in the world. It was a dumb thing to do. The
experiment had no immediate practical application, while the journal
accepted only clinical reports. Moreover, the publication was very political; normally you had to have an established reputation or come from
one of the big orthopedic programs, like Harvard or Columbia, to get
into it. Luckily, I didn't know that. Someone thought my paper was
just what the doctor ordered. Not only was it accepted for publication,
but I was invited to present it at the next combined meeting of the
Orthopaedic Research Society and the American Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons, at Miami Beach in January 1961. This invitation was a
particular honor, for it meant someone considered my work so significant that practicing physicians, as well as researchers, should hear of it
right then and there. Whoever that someone was, he or she has my
undying gratitude.
My report was well received and soon was published, to the consternation of the local inquisitors and the delight of Chester Yntema.
Since the journal was geared to clinicians. I worried that my experiment
wouldn't reach the basic researchers with whom I really wanted to share
it, but again I was wrong. The next year I got a phone call from Meryl Rose himself. He was excited by the article and wanted to know what
I'd done since.
Although Rose taught at Tulane Medical School in New Orleans, he
spent every summer at the Woods Hole Marine Biological Laboratory on
Cape Cod, so he and his wife drove to Syracuse from there. Despite his
success, Rose had maintained the completely open mind that a great
researcher must have, and he was fascinated by the observations on electric fields, nerves, anesthesia, and magnetism that I'll recount in the
next chapter. Since then his interest has encouraged me enormously. My
friendship with this fine man and scientist has been fruitful even beyond
the expectations I had then, and, when my wife and I had the Roses to
dinner, we found our pasts were linked by an odd coincidence. As they
walked in the door, Lillian exclaimed, "Dr. Rose! Weren't you at Smith
College in the 1940s?" It turned out that she'd been a friend of Rose's
student lab assistant and had helped catch the frogs for the famous salt in-the-wound experiment!
Four
Life's Potentials
It's an axiom of science that the better an experiment is, the more new
questions it raises after it has answered the one you asked. By that standard my first simple test had been pretty good. The new problems
branched out like the fingers on those restored limbs: Where did the
injury currents come from? Were they in fact related to the nervous
system and, if so, how? It seemed unlikely that they sprang into action
only after an amputation; they must have existed before. There must
have been a preexisting substratum of direct current activity that responded to the injury. Did the voltages I measured really reflect such
currents, and did they flow throughout the salamander's body? Did
other organisms have them? What structures carried them? What were
their electrical properties? What were they doing the rest of the time,
before injury and after healing? Could they be used to provoke regeneration where it was normally absent?
I had ideas about how to look for some of the answers, but, to understand my approach, the reader unfamiliar with electrical terms will need
a simplified explanation of several basic concepts that are essential to the
rest of the story.
Everything electrical stems from the phenomenon of charge. No one
knows exactly what this is, except to say that it's a fundamental property
of matter that exists in two opposite forms, or polarities, which we
arbitrarily call positive and negative. Protons, which are one of the two
main types of particles in atomic nuclei, are positive; the other particles,
the neutrons, are so named because they have no charge. Orbiting
around the nucleus are electrons, in the same number as the protons inside the nucleus.
Although an electron is 1,836 times less massive
than a proton, the electron carries an equal but opposite (negative)
charge. Because of their lightness and their position outside the nucleus,
electrons are much more easily dislodged from atoms than are protons,
so they're the main carriers of electric charge. For the lay person's purposes a negative charge can be thought of as a surplus of electrons, while
a positive charge can be considered a scarcity of them. When electrons
move away from an area, it becomes positively charged, and the area to
which they move becomes negative.
A flow of electrons is called a current, and is measured in amperes,
units named for an early-nineteenth-century French physicist, Andre
Marie Ampere. A direct current is a more or less even flow, as opposed
to the instantaneous discharge of static electricity as sparks or lightning,
or the back-and-forth flow of alternating current which powers most of
our appliances.
Besides the amount of charge being moved, a current has another
characteristic important for our narrative—its electromotive force. This
can be visualized as the "push" behind the current, and it's measured in
volts (named for Alessandro Volta).
In high school most of us learned that a current flows only when a
source of electrons (negatively charged material) is connected to a material having fewer free electrons (positively charged in relation to the
source) by a conductor, through which the electrons can flow. This is
what happens when you connect the negative terminal of a battery to its
positive pole with a wire or a radio's innards: You've completed a circuit
between negative and positive. If there's no conductor, and hence no
circuit, there's only a hypothetical charge flow, or electric potential,
between the two areas. The force of this latent current is also measured
in volts by temporarily completing the circuit with a recording device,
as I did in my experiment.
The potential can continue to build until a violent burst of current
equalizes the charges; this is what happens when lightning strikes.
Smaller potentials may remain stable, however. In this case they must be
continuously fed by a direct current flowing from positive to negative,
the opposite of the normal direction. In this part of a circuit, electrons
actually flow from where they're scarce to where they're more abundant.
As Volta found, such a flow is generated inside a battery by the electrical
interaction of two metals.
An electric field forms around any electric charge. This means that
any other charged object will be attracted (if the polarities are opposite)
or repelled (if they're the same) for a certain distance around the first object. The field is the region of space in which an electrical charge can
be detected, and it's measured in volts per unit of area.
Electric fields must be distinguished from magnetic fields. Like
charge, magnetism is a dimly understood intrinsic property of matter
that manifests itself in two polarities. Any flow of electrons sets up a
combined electric and magnetic field around the current, which in turn
affects other electrons nearby. Around a direct current the electromagnetic field is stable, whereas an alternating current's field collapses and
reappears with its poles reversed every time the current changes direction. This reversal happens sixty times a second in our normal house
currents. Just as a current produces a magnetic field, a magnetic field,
when it moves in relation to a conductor, induces a current. Any varying
magnetic field, like that around household appliances, generates a current in nearby conductors. The weak magnetic fields we'll be discussing
are measured in gauss, units named after a nineteenth-century German
pioneer in the study of magnetism, Karl Friedrich Gauss.
Both electric and magnetic fields are really just abstractions that scientists have made up to try to understand electricity's and magnetism's
action at a distance, produced by no known intervening material or energy, a phenomenon that used to be considered impossible until it became undeniable. A field is represented by lines of force, another
abstraction, to indicate its direction and shape. Both kinds of fields decline with distance, but their influence is technically infinite: Every time
you use your toaster, the fields around it perturb charged particles in the
farthest galaxies ever so slightly.
In addition, there's a whole universe full of electromagnetic energy,
radiation that somehow seems to be both waves in an electromagnetic
field and particles at the same time. It exists in a spectrum of wavelengths that includes cosmic rays, gamma rays, X rays, ultraviolet radiation, visible light, infrared radiation, microwaves, and radio waves.
Together, electromagnetic fields and energies interact in many complex
ways that have given rise to much of the natural world, not to mention
the whole technology of electronics.
You'll need a casual acquaintance with all these terms for the story
ahead, but don't worry if the concepts seem a bit murky. Physicists have
been trying for generations to solve the fundamental mysteries of electromagnetism, and no one, not even Einstein, has yet succeeded.
Unpopular Science
None of these things had the slightest relevance to life, according to
most biologists around 1960. A major evaluation of American medicine,
financed by the Carnegie Foundation and published in 1910 by the respected educator Abraham Flexner, had denounced the clinical use of
electric shocks and currents, which had been applied, often overenthusiastically, to many diseases since the mid-1700s. Electrotherapy
sometimes seemed to work, but no one knew why, and it had gotten a
bad name from the many charlatans who'd exploited it. Its legitimate
proponents had no scientific way to defend it, so the reforms in medical
education that followed the Flexner report drove all mention of it from
the classroom and clinic, just as the last remnants of belief in vital electricity were being purged from biology by the discovery of acetylcholine.
This development dovetailed nicely with expanding knowledge of biochemistry and growing reliance on the drug industry's products. Penicillin later made medicine almost exclusively drug oriented.
Meanwhile, the work of Faraday, Edison, Marconi, and others literally electrified the world. As the uses of electricity multiplied, no one
found any obvious effects on living creatures except for the shock and
heating caused by large currents. To be sure, no one looked very hard,
for fear of discouraging a growth industry, but the magic of electricity
seemed to lie precisely in the way it worked its wonders unseen and
unfelt by the folks clustered around the radio or playing cards under the
light bulb. By the 1920s, no scientist intent on a respectable career
dared suggest that life was in any sense electrical.
Nevertheless, some researchers kept coming up with observations that
didn't fit the prevailing view. Although their work was mostly consigned to the fringes of the scientific community, by the late 1950s
they'd accumulated quite a bit of evidence.
There were two groups of dissenters, but, because their work went
unheeded, each was largely unaware of the other's existence. One line of
inquiry began just after the turn of the century when it was learned that
hydras were electrically polarized. The head was found to be positive,
the tail negative. I've already mentioned Frazee's 1909 report of salamander regeneration enhanced by electrical currents. Then, with a classic series of experiments in the early 1920s, Elmer J. Lund of the
University of Texas found that the polarity of regeneration in species
related to the hydra could be controlled, even reversed, by small direct
currents passes through the animal's body. A current strong enough to override the creature's normal polarity could cause a head to form where
a tail should have reappeared, and vice versa. Others confirmed this discovery, and Lund went on to study eggs and embryos. He claimed to
have influenced the development of frog eggs not only with currents but
also with magnetic fields, a conclusion that was really risque for that
time.
Stimulated by Lund's papers, Harold Saxton Burr of Yale began putting electrodes to all kinds of creatures. Burr was lucky enough to have a
forum for his work. He edited the Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine,
where most of his reports appeared; few other journals would touch
them. Burr and his co-workers found electric fields around, and electric
potentials on the surfaces of, organisms as diverse as worms, hydras,
salamanders, humans, other mammals, and even slime molds. They
measured changes in these potentials and correlated them to growth,
regeneration, tumor formation, drug effects, hypnosis, and sleep. Burr
claimed to have measured field changes resulting from ovulation, but
others got contradictory results. He hooked up his voltmeters to trees for
years at a time and found that their fields varied in response not only to
light and moisture, but to storms, sunspots, and the phases of the moon
as well.
Burr and Lund were handicapped by their instruments as well as the
research climate. Most of their work was done before World War II and,
even though Burr spent years designing the most sensitive devices possible using vacuum tubes, the meters were still too "noisy" to reliably
measure the tiny currents found in living things. The two scientists
could refine their observations only enough to find a simple dipolar distribution of potentials, the head of most animals being negative and the
tail positive.
Burr and Lund advanced similar theories of an electrodynamic field,
called by Burr the field of life or L-field, which held the shape of an
organism just as a mold determines the shape of a gelatin dessert.
"When we meet a friend we have not seen for six months there is not
one molecule in his face which was there when we last saw him," Burr
wrote. "But, thanks to his controlling L-field, the new molecules have
fallen into the old, familiar pattern and we can recognize his face."
Burr believed that faults in the field could reveal latent illness just as
dents in a mold show up in the jelly. He claimed to be able to predict
all sorts of things about a person's emotional and physical health, both
present and future, merely by checking the voltage between head and
hand. His later writings were marred by a son of bioelectric determinism and a tendency to confuse "law and order" in nature with that odious euphemism as preached by Presidents. As a result, he began to
suggest his simple readings as a foolproof way to evaluate job applicants,
soldiers, mental patients, and suspected criminals or dissidents.
The fields Burr and Lund found were actually far too simple to account for a salamander's limb or a human face. Biological knowledge at
that time gave them no theoretical framework to explain where their
fields came from. They conceived of currents flowing within cells but
had no proof. They had no inkling that currents might flow in specific
tissues or in the fluids outside cells. They suggested that all these little
intracellular currents somehow added up to the whole field. Burr wrote
that "electrical energy is a fundamental attribute of protoplasm and is an
expression or measure of the presence of an electrodynamic field in the
organism."
Unfortunately, an analysis of this sentence yields nonsense,
and Burr's work was dismissed as foggy vitalism. Lund suffered the same
fate. No one bothered to see if the measurements they'd made were valid.
After all, you can disagree with a theory, but you should respect the
data enough to check them. If you can't duplicate them, you're entitled
to rest easy with your own concepts, but if you get the same results,
you're obligated to agree or propose an alternate theory. Most scientists
took the easy way out, however, and simply ignored Burr and Lund.
Their discoveries remained little known, and most biologists didn't connect them with the tentative morphogenetic-field concept of regeneration.
Then in 1952 Lund's work was taken up by G. Marsh and H. W.
Beams using the planarian. They found that the flatworm's polarity, like
the hydra's, could be controlled by passing a current through it. When a
direct current was fed in the proper direction through a section of a
worm, normal polarity disappeared and a head formed at each end. As
the current strength was increased, the section's polarity reversed; a head
regrew at the rear, a tail at the front. At higher voltages, even intact
worms completely reorganized, with the head becoming a tail and vice
versa. Marsh and Beams grew convinced that the animal's electric field
was the morphogenetic organizing principle. Still, their work was also
ignored, except by Meryl Rose, who suggested that a gradation of electrical charge from front to back controlled the gradient of growth inhibitors and stimulators. He suggested that the growth compounds were
charged molecules that were moved to different places in the body by
the electric field, depending on the amount and sign of their charge and
their molecular weight.
Undercurrents in Neurology
While the investigation of the total body field moved haltingly forward in the study of simple animals, several neurophysiologists began
finding out odd things about the nerves of more complex creatures, data
that Bernstein's action potential couldn't explain. Going through the old
literature, following lead after lead from one paper to the next, I found
many hints that there were DC potentials in the nervous system and that
small currents from outside could affect brain function.
The first recorded use of currents on the nervous system was by
Giovanni Aldini, a nephew of Galvani and an ardent champion of vitalism. Using the batteries of his archenemy Volta, Aldini claimed remarkable success in relieving asthma. He also cured a man who today
would probably be diagnosed as schizophrenic, although it's impossible
to know how much benefit came from the currents and how much from
simple solicitude, then so rare in treating mental illness. Aldini gave his
patient a room in his own house and later found him a job. Some of
Aldini's experiments were grotesque - he tried to resurrect recently executed criminals by making the corpses twitch with electricity - but his idea that external current could replenish the vital force of exhausted
nerves became the rationale for a whole century of electrotherapy.
Modern studies of nerves and current began in 1902, when French
researcher Stephane Leduc reported putting animals to sleep by passing
fairly strong alternating currents through their heads. He even knocked
himself unconscious several times by this method. (Talk about dedication to science!) Several others took up this lead in the 1930s and developed the techniques of electroshock and electronarcosis. The therapeutic
value of using large currents to produce convulsions has been questioned
more and more, until now it's mostly used to quiet unmanageable psychotics and political nonconformists. Electronarcosis—induction of sleep
by passing small currents across the head from temple to temple—is
widely used by legitimate therapists in France and the Soviet Union.
Russian doctors claim their elektroson technique, which uses electrodes on
the eyelids and behind the ears to deliver weak direct currents pulsing at
calmative brain-wave frequencies, can impart the benefits of a full
night's sleep in two or three hours. There's still much dispute about how
both techniques work, but from the outset there was no denying that
the currents had a profound effect on the nervous system.
In the second and third decades of this century there was a flurry of
interest in galvanotaxis, the idea that direct currents guided the growth
of cells, especially neurons. In 1920, S. Ingvar found that the fibers
growing out of nerve cell bodies would align themselves with a nearby
flow of current and that the fibers growing toward the negative electrode
were different from those growing toward the positive one. Paul Weiss
soon "explained" this heretical observation as an artifact caused by
stretching of the cell culture substrate due to contact with the electrodes. Even after Marsh and Beams proved Weiss wrong in 1946, it
took many more years for the scientific community to accept the fact
that neuron fibers do orient themselves along a current flow. Today the
possible use of electricity to guide nerve growth is one of the most exciting prospects in regeneration research (see Chapter 11).
The Bernstein hypothesis, unable to account for these facts, has
turned out to be deficient in several other respects. To begin with, according to the theory, an impulse should travel with equal ease in either
direction along the nerve fiber. If the nerve is stimulated in the middle,
an impulse should travel in both directions to opposite ends. Instead,
impulses travel only in one direction; in experiments they can be made
to travel "upstream," but only with great difficulty. This may not seem
like such a big deal, but it is very significant. Something seems to polarize the nerve.
Another problem is the fact that, although nerves are essential for
regeneration, the action potentials are silent during the process. No impulses have ever been found to be related to regrowth, and neurotransmitters such as acetylcholine have been ruled out as growth
stimulators.
In addition, impulses always have the same magnitude and speed.
This may not seem like such a big thing either, but think about it. It
means the nerve can carry only one message, like the digital computer's
1 or 0. This is okay for simple things like the knee-jerk reflex. When
the doctor's rubber hammer taps your knee, it's actually striking the
patellar tendon, giving it a quick stretch. This stimulates stretch receptors (nerve cells in the tendon), which fire a signal to the spinal cord
saying, "The patellar tendon has suddenly been stretched." These impulses are received by motor (muscle-activating) neurons in the spinal
cord, which send impulses to the large muscle on the front of the thigh,
telling it to contract and straighten the leg. In everyday life, the reflex
keeps you from falling in a heap if an outside force suddenly bends your
knees.
The digital impulse system accounts for this perfectly well. However,
no one can walk on reflexes alone, as victims of cerebral palsy know all
too well. The motor activities we take for granted—getting out of a
chair and walking across a room, picking up a cup and drinking coffee,
and so on—require integration of all the muscles and sensory organs
working smoothly together to produce coordinated movements that we
don't even have to think about. No one has ever explained how the
simple code of impulses can do all that.
Even more troublesome are the
higher processes, such as sight—in which somehow we interpret a constantly changing scene made of innumerable bits of visual data—or the
speech patterns, symbol recognition, and grammar of our languages.
Heading the list of riddles is the "mind-brain problem" of consciousness, with its recognition, "I am real; I think; I am something
special." Then there are abstract thought, memory, personality,
creativity, and dreams. The story goes that Otto Loewi had wrestled
with the problem of the synapse for a long time without result, when
one night he had a dream in which the entire frog-heart experiment was
revealed to him. When he awoke, he knew he'd had the dream, but he'd
forgotten the details.
The next night he had the same dream. This time
he remembered the procedure, went to his lab in the morning, did the
experiment, and solved the problem. The inspiration that seemed to
banish neural electricity forever can't be explained by the theory it supported! How do you convert simple digital messages into these complex phenomena? Latter-day mechanists have simply postulated brain circuitry so intricate that we will probably never figure it out, but some
scientists have said there must be other factors.
Even as Loewi was finishing his work on acetylcholine, others began
to find evidence that currents flowed in the nerves. English physiologist
Richard Caton had already claimed he'd detected an electric field around
the heads of animals in 1875, but it wasn't until 1924 that German
psychiatrist Hans Berger proved it by recording the first electroencephalogram (EEG) from platinum wires he inserted into his son's scalp.
The EEG provided a record of rhythmic fluctuations in potential voltage
over various parts of the head. Berger at first thought there was only one
wave from the whole brain, but it soon became clear that the waves
differed, depending on where the electrodes were put. Modern EEGs use
as many as thirty-two separate channels, all over the head.
The frequency of these brain waves has been crudely correlated with
states of consciousness. Delta waves (0.5 to 3 cycles per second) indicate
deep sleep. Theta waves (4 to 8 cycles per second) indicate trance, drowsiness, or light sleep. Alpha waves (8 to 14 cycles per second) appear
during relaxed wakefulness or meditation. And beta waves (14 to 35
cycles per second), the most uneven forms, accompany all the modulations of our active everyday consciousness. Underlying these rhythms are
potentials that vary much more slowly, over periods as long as several
minutes. Today's EEG machines are designed to filter them out because
they cause the trace to wander and are considered insignificant anyway.
There's still no consensus as to where the EEG voltages come from.
They would be most easily explained by direct currents, both steady
state and pulsing, throughout the brain, but that has been impossible
for most biologists to accept. The main alternative theory, that large
numbers of neurons firing simultaneously can mimic real electrical activity, has never been proven.
In 1939, W. E. Burge of the University of Illinois found that the
voltage measured between the head and other parts of the body became
more negative during physical activity, declined in sleep, and reversed
to positive under general anesthesia. At about the same time a group of
physiologists and neurologists at Harvard Medical School began studying the brain with a group of MIT mathematicians. This association was
destined to change the world. From it came many of our modern concepts of cybernetics, and it became the nucleus of the main American
task force on computers in World War II. One of the group's first important ideas was that the brain worked by a combination of analog and
digital coding.
One of the mathematicians, computer pioneer John von Neumann,
later elaborated the concept in great detail, but basically it's rather simple. In analog computers, changes in information are expressed by analogous changes in the magnitude or polarity of a current. For example, if
the computer is to use and store the varying temperatures of a furnace,
the rise and fall in heat can be mimicked by a rise and fall in voltage.
Analog systems are slow and can handle only simple information, but
they can express subtle variations very well. Digital coding, on the other
hand, can transmit enormous amounts of data at high speed, but only if
the information can be reduced to yes-no, on-off bits—the digits 1 and
0.
If the brain was such a hybrid computer, these early cyberneticist
reasoned, then analog coding could control the overall activity of large
groups of neurons by such actions as increasing or decreasing their sensitivity to incoming messages. (A few years later neurologists did find
that some neurons were "tuned" to fire only if they received a certain
number of impulses.) The digital system would transfer sensory and
motor information, but the processing of that information—memory
and recall, thought, and so on—would be accomplished by the synergism of both methods. The voltage changes Burge found in response
to major alterations of consciousness seemed to fit within this framework, and his observations were extended by the Harvard-MIT group
and others. Much of this work was done directly on the exposed brains
of animals and of human patients during surgery. When cooperative
patients elected to remain awake during such operations (the brain is
immune to pain), human sensations could often be correlated with electrical data. Contributors to this endeavor included nearly all of the
greatest American neurophysiologists—Walter B. Cannon, Arturo Rosenblueth, Ralph Gerard, Gilbert Ling, Wilder Penfield, and others.
Measurements on the exposed brain quickly confirmed the existence of
potential voltages and also revealed possible currents of injury. Whenever groups of nerve cells were actively conducting impulses, they also
produced a negative potential. Positive potentials appeared from injured
cells when the brain had been damaged; these potentials then expanded
outward to uninjured cells, suppressing their ability to send or receive
impulses. When experimenters applied small negative voltages to groups
of neurons, their sensitivity increased; that is, they would generate an
impulse in response to a weaker stimulus. Externally applied positive
potentials worked in the opposite way: They depressed nerve function,
making it harder to produce an impulse. Thus there did seem to be an
analog code, but how did it work? Did the potentials come from direct
currents generated by the nerve cells themselves, or did they merely result from adding up a lot of action potentials all going in the same
direction and arriving in the same place at the same time?
Some answers were provided by a series of beautiful experiments by
Ling, Gerard, and Benjamin Libet at the University of Chicago. Working on frogs, they studied areas of the cortex where the neuron layer was
only one cell thick and the cells were arranged side by side like soldiers
on review, all pointing in the same direction. In such areas they found a
negative potential on the dendrites (the short incoming fibers) and a
positive potential at the ends of the axons (the longer outgoing fibers).
This indicated a steady direct current along the normal direction of impulse transmission. The entire nerve cell was electrically polarized.
In another series of experiments, on brains removed from frogs and
kept alive in culture, the Chicago group found that direct currents swept
across the surface of the cortex in very slow waves, which could be produced experimentally by applying chemicals such as caffeine to a single
spot on the surface. When they made a cut on the brain, severing groups
of nerve fibers, these DC traveling waves would still cross the cut if the
two surfaces were in direct contact. If the researchers held the cut open and
filled it with a saline solution that matched body fluids, then the waves
couldn't cross the gap. These were particularly important observations.
They indicated that the current was transmitted by structures outside the
neurons; it crossed the cut when the edges touched, but the microscopic
parts of the severed neurons wouldn't have rejoined so easily. The results
also showed that the current was not a flow of ions; otherwise it would
have been able to cross the gap through the salt water.
Studying intact brains in living frogs, the lame group found a potential between the front and back of the brain. The olfactory (frontal) lobes
were several milivolts negative with respect to the occipital (rear) lobe, implying a current flowing up the brain stem and between the two
hemispheres to the front.
At the time, these observations seemed mighty odd. They didn't fit
any concepts of how the nerves worked. As a result, they were largely
ignored. The majority of neurophysiologists went on measuring the action potentials and tracing out fiber pathways in the brain. This was
useful work but limited. The basic questions remained.
Only one research team followed up this work, some ten years later.
Sidney Goldring and James L. O'Leary, neuropsychiatrists at the Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis, recorded the same
DC potentials from the human scalp, from the exposed brain during
surgery, and from the brains of monkeys and rabbits. As noted before,
the potentials varied in regular cycles several minutes long, like a basso
continuo under the EEG. In fact, Goldring and O'Leary found waves
within waves: "Written upon the slow major swings were lesser voltage
changes." These were weak potentials, measured in microvolts (millionths of a volt) and varying in waves of 2 to 30 cycles per minute, sort
of a pianissimo "inner voice" in a three-part electrical fugue.
Conducting in a New Mode
I was acutely aware that I didn't have the "proper" background for the
work I planned to do. I wasn't a professional neurophysiologist; I didn't
even know one. Indeed, after my run-in with the research committee,
one member had taken me aside and earnestly advised me, "Go back to
school and get your Ph.D., Becker. Then you'll learn all of this stuff is
nonsense." Still, some of the greatest neurophysiologists had thought
the same way I did about "all of this stuff." They suggested we might
have been too hasty in throwing electric currents out of biology. My
notion of putting them back in wasn't so outlandish, but only an extension of what they'd been saying.
I was approaching the body's system of
information transfer from the periphery, asking, "What makes wounds
heal?" They'd started from the center, asking, "How does the brain
work?" We were working on the same problem from opposite ends. As I
contemplated their findings and all of biology's unsolved problems, I
grew convinced that life was more complex than we suspected. I felt that
those who reduced life to a mechanical interaction of molecules were
living in a cold, gray, dead world, which, despite its drabness, was a
fantasy. I didn't think electricity would turn to be any elan vital in
the old sense, but I had a hunch would be closer to the secret than the smells of the biochemistry lab or the dissecting room's preserved organs.
I had another worthy ally when I started to reevaluate the role of
electricity in life. Albert Szent-Gyorgyi, who'd already won a Nobel
Prize for his work on oxidation and vitamin C, made a stunning suggestion in a speech before the Budapest Academy of Science on March 21,
1941. (Think of the date. World War II was literally exploding around
him, and there he was, calmly laying the foundations for a new biology.)
Speaking of the mechanistic approach of biochemistry, he pointed out
that when experimenters broke living things down into their constituent
parts, somewhere along the line life slipped through their fingers and
they found themselves working with dead matter. He said, "It looks as
if some basic fact about life were still missing, without which any real
understanding is impossible." For the missing basic fact, Szent-Gyorgyi
proposed putting electricity back into living things, but not in the way
it had been thought of at the turn of the century.
At that earlier time, there had been only two known modes of current
conduction, ionic and metallic. Metallic conduction can be visualized as
a cloud of electrons moving along the surface of metal, usually a wire. It
can be automatically excluded from living creatures because no one has
ever found any wires in them. Ionic current is conducted in solutions by
the movement of ions—atoms or molecules charged by having more or
fewer than the number of electrons needed to balance their protons'
positive charges. Since ions are much bigger than electrons, they move
more laboriously through the conducting medium, and ionic currents
die out after short distances. They work fine across the thin membrane of
the nerve fiber, but it would be impossible to sustain an ionic current
down the length of even the shortest nerve.
Semi-conduction, the third kind of current, was a laboratory curiosity
in the 1930s. Halfway between conductors and insulators, the semiconductors are inefficient, in the sense that they can carry only small currents, but they can conduct their currents readily over long distances.
Without them, modern computers, satellites, and all the rest of our
solid-state electronics would be impossible.
Semi-conduction occurs only in materials having an orderly molecular
structure, such as crystals, in which electrons can move easily from the
electron cloud around one atomic nucleus to the cloud around another.
The atoms in a crystal are arranged in neat geometric lattices, rather
than the frozen jumble of ordinary solids. Some crystalline materials
have spaces in the lattice where other atoms can fit. The atoms of these
impurities may have more or fewer electrons than the atoms of the lattice material.
Since the forces of the latticework structure hold the same number of electrons in place around each atom, the "extra" electrons of
the impurity atoms are free to move through the lattice without being
bound to any particular atom. If the impurity atoms have fewer electrons
than the others, the "holes" in their electron clouds can be filled by
electrons from other atoms, leaving holes elsewhere. A negative current,
or N-type semi-conduction, amounts to the movement of excess electrons; a positive current, or P-type semi-conduction, is the movement of
these holes, which can be thought of as positive charges.
Szent-Gyorgyi pointed out that the molecular structure of many parts
of the cell was regular enough to support semi-conduction. This idea was
almost completely ignored at the time. Even when Szent-Gyorgyi expanded the concept in his I960 Introduction to a Submolecular Biology,
most scientists (except in Russia!) dismissed it as evidence of his advancing age, but that little book was an inspiration to me. I think it may
turn out to be the man's most important contribution to science.
In it
he conjectured that protein molecules, each having a sort of slot or way
station for mobile electrons, might be joined together in long chains so
that electrons could flow in a semiconducting current over long distances
without losing energy, much as in a game of checkers one counter could
jump along a row of other pieces across the entire board. Szent-Gyorgyi
suggested that the electron flow would be similar to photosynthesis,
another process he helped elucidate, in which a kind of waterfall of electrons cascaded step by step down a staircase of molecules, losing energy
with each bounce. The main difference was that in protein semi-conduction the electrons' energy would be conserved and passed along as information instead of being absorbed and stored in the chemical bonds of
food.
With Szent-Gyorgyi's suggestion in mind, I put together my working
hypothesis. I postulated a primitive, analog-coded information system
that was closely related to the nerves but not necessarily located in the
nerve fibers themselves. I theorized that this system used semiconducting direct currents and that, either alone or in concert with the nerve
impulse system, it regulated growth, healing, and perhaps other basic
processes.
Testing the Concept
The first order of business was to repeat Burr's measurements on salamanders, using modern equipment. I put the reference electrode at the
tip of each animal's nose and moved the recording electrode point by
point along the center of the body to the tip of the tail, and then out
along each limb. I measured voltages on the rest of the body and plotted
lines of force connecting all the points where the readings were the
same.
Instead of Burr's simple head-negative and tail-positive form, I found
a complex field that followed the arrangement of the nervous system.
There were large positive potentials over each lobe of the brain, and
slightly smaller ones over the brachial and lumbar nerve ganglia between
each pair of limbs. The readings grew increasingly negative as I moved
away from these collections of nerve cell bodies; the hands, feet, and tip
of the tail had the highest negative potentials.
In another series of measurements, I watched the potentials develop
along with the nervous system in larval salamanders. In the adults, cutting the nerves where they entered the legs—that is, severing the long
nerve fibers from their cell bodies in the spinal cord—wiped out the
limb potentials almost entirely. But if I cut the spinal cord, leaving the
peripheral nerves connected to their cell bodies, the limb potentials
didn't change. It certainly looked as though there was a current being
generated in the nerve cell bodies and traveling down the fibers.
To have a current flow you need a circuit; the current has to be made
at one spot, pass through a conductor, and eventually get back to the
generator. We tend to forget that the 60-cycle alternating current in the wall socket isn't used up when we turn on a light but is merely coursing
through it to the ground, through which it eventually returns to the
power station. Since my measurements were positive over collections of
nerve cell bodies, and increasingly negative out along the nerve fibers, it
seemed a good bet that current was being generated in the cell bodies,
especially since they contained all of the "good stuff"—the nucleus,
organelles, and metabolic components—while the fibers were relatively
uninteresting prolongations of the body. At the time, I supposed the
circuit was completed by current going back toward the spine through
the muscles.
This was a good start, but it wasn't scientifically acceptable proof. For
one thing, my guess about the return part of the circuit was soon disproved when I measured the limb muscles and found them polarized in
the same direction as the surface potentials. For another thing, it had
recently been discovered that amphibian skin itself was polarized, inside
versus outside, by ion differences much like the nerve membrane's resting potential, so it was just barely possible that my readings had been caused by ionic discharges through the moist skin. If so, my evidence
was literally all wet.
Much of the uncertainty was due to the fact that I was measuring the
outside of the animal and assuming that generators and conductors inside
were making the pattern I found. I needed a way to relate inner currents
to outer potentials.
This was before transistors had entirely replaced vacuum tubes. A
tube's characteristics depended on the structure of the electric field inside it, but to calculate the field parameters in advance without computers was a laborious task, so radio engineers often made an analog model.
They built a large mock-up of the tube, filled with a conducting solution. When current was applied to the model, the field could be mapped
by measuring the voltage at various points in the solution. I decided to
build a model salamander.
I made an analog of the creature's nervous system out of copper wires.
For the brain and nerve ganglia I used blobs of solder. Each junction was
thus a voltaic battery of two different metals, copper and the lead-tin
alloy of which the solder was made. Then I simply sandwiched this
"nervous system" between two pieces of sponge rubber cut in the shape
of a salamander, and soaked the model in a salt solution to approximate
body fluids and serve as the electrolyte, the conducting solution that
would enable the two metals to function as a battery. It worked. The
readings were almost exactly the same as in the real salamander. This
showed that a direct current inside could produce the potentials I was
getting on the outside.
If my proposed system was really a primitive part of the nervous system, it should be widely distributed, so next I surveyed the whole animal kingdom. I tested flatworms, earthworms, fish, amphibians,
reptiles, mammals, and humans. In each species the potentials on the
skin reflected the arrangement of the nervous system. In the worms and
fish, there was only one area of positive potential, just as there was only
one major nerve ganglion, the brain. In humans the entire head and
spinal region, with its massive concentration of neurons, was strongly
positive.
The three specific areas of greatest positive potential were the
same as in the salamander: the brain, the brachial plexus between the
shoulder blades, and the lumbar enlargement at the base of the spinal
cord. In all vertebrates I also recorded a midline head potential that
suggested a direct current like that postulated by Gerard, flowing from
back to front through the middle of the brain. It looked as though the
current came from the reticular activating system, a network of crosslinked neurons that fanned out from the brainstem into higher centers and seemed to control the level of sleep or wakefulness and the focus of
attention.
At the same time, to see whether the current of injury and the surface
potentials came from the same source, I made electrical measurements
on salamander limbs as they healed fractures. (As mentioned in Chapter
1, bone healing is the only kind of true regeneration common to all
vertebrates.) The limb currents behaved like those accompanying regrowth. A positive zone immediately formed around the break, although
the rest of the limb retained at least part of its negative potential. Then,
between the fifth and tenth days, the positive zone reversed its potential
and became more strongly negative than the rest of the limb as the
fracture began to heal.
Next I decided to follow up Burge's experiments of two decades before. I would produce various changes in the state of the nervous system
and look for concomitant changes in the electrical measurements. To do
this right I really needed a few thousand dollars for an apparatus that
could take readings from several electrodes simultaneously and record
them side by side on a chart. My chances of getting this money seemed slim unless I could publish another paper fast. I decided to use the
equipment I had for a simple measurement during one of the most profound changes in consciousness—anesthesia.
Burge was right. The electrical responses were dramatic and incontrovertible. As each animal went under, its peripheral voltages
dropped to zero, and in very deep anesthesia they reversed to some extent, the limbs and tail going positive. They reverted to normal just
before the animal woke up.
I had enough for a short paper, and I decided to try a journal on
medical electronics recently started by the Institute of Radio Engineers.
Although most of what they printed was safe and unremarkable, I'd
found that engineers were often more open-minded than biologists, so I
went for broke; I put in the whole hypothesis—analog nervous system,
semiconducting currents, healing control, the works. The editor loved it
and sent me an enthusiastic letter of acceptance, along with suggestions
for further research. Best of all, I soon got another small grant approved
and bought my multi-electrode chart recorder. Soon I had confirmed my anesthesia findings, and with the whole-body monitoring setup I also
was able to correlate the entire pattern of surface voltages with the animal's level of activity while not anesthetized. Negative potentials in the
brain's frontal area and at the periphery of the nervous system were associated with wakefulness, sensory stimuli, and muscle movements. The
more activity, the greater the negative potentials were. A shift toward
the positive occurred during rest and even more so during sleep. *
*l didn't know it until later, but another experimenter named H. Caspers made similar findings at about the same time.
In my reading on solid-state electronics I found another way I could
test for current in the salamander. Luckily it was cheap and easy; I could
do it without buying more equipment. Best of all, it should work only
if the current was semiconducting.
Suppose you think you have a current flowing through some conductor—a salamander's limb, for instance. You put it in a strong magnetic
field so that the lines of force cut across the conductor at right angles.
Then you place another conductor, containing no current, perpendicular
to both the original conductor (the limb) and the magnetic field. If there
is a current in whatever you're testing, some of the charge carriers will
be deflected by the magnetic field into the other conductor, producing a
voltage that you can measure. This is called the Hall voltage, after the
gentleman who discovered it. The beauty of it is that it works differently for the three kinds of current. For any given strength of magnetic field, the Hall voltage is proportional to the mobility of the charge
carriers. Ions in a solution are relatively big and barely deflected by the
field. Electrons in a wire are constrained by the nature of the metal. In
both cases the Hall voltage is small and hard to detect. Electrons in
semiconductors are very free to move, however, and produce Hall voltages with much weaker fields.
After finding a C-shaped permanent magnet, an item not much in
demand since the advent of electromagnets, I set up the equipment. I
took a deep breath as I placed the first anesthetized salamander on its
plastic support, with one foreleg extended. I'd placed electrodes so that
they touched the limb lightly, one on each side, and I'd mounted the
magnet so as to swing in with its poles above and below the limb, close
to yet not touching it. I took voltage measurements every few minutes,
with the magnet and without it, as the animal regained consciousness. I
also measured the DC voltage from the tips of the fingers to the spinal
cord. In deep anesthesia, the DC voltage along the limb was zero and so
was the Hall voltage. As the anesthetic wore off, the normal potential
along the limb gradually appeared, and so did a beautiful Hall voltage.
It increased right along with the limb potential, until the animal recovered completely and walked away from the apparatus. The test
worked every time, but I don't think I'll ever forget the thrill of watching the pen on the recorder trace out the first of those Hall voltages.
This experiment demonstrated unequivocally that there was a real
electric current flowing along the salamander's foreleg, and it virtually
proved that the current was semiconducting. In fact, the half-dozen tests
I'd performed supported every point of my hypothesis.
Scientific results that aren't reported might as well not exist. They're
like the sound of one hand clapping. For scientists, communication isn't
only a responsibility, it's our chief pleasure. A good result from a clean,
beautiful experiment is a joy that you just have to share, and I couldn't
wait to see these data in print. I went for the top this time. The journal
in American science is aptly named Science. Each issue reports on all
fields from astronomy to zoology, so publication means a paper has more
than a specialized significance. Mine was accepted, and I was jubilant.
With three major papers in three major journals after my first year of
research, I felt I'd arrived. The world has a way of cutting you down to
size, however, and in the science game the method is known as citation.
No matter how important your paper is, it doesn't mean anything unless
it's cited as a reference in new papers by others and you get a respectable
number of requests for reprints. On both counts, I was a failure. I was
learning how science treats new ideas that conflict with old ones.
I didn't stay discouraged long, though. I was doing science for the
love of it, not for praise. I felt the concepts emerging from my reading
and research were important, and I was passionately committed to testing them. I knew that if the results were ever to change any minds, I
would have to be careful not to misinterpret data. In going deeper into
the electrical properties of nerves, I realized, I was about to get over my
head in an area I really wasn't trained for—physics. I made one of the
best decisions of my life; I looked for a collaborator.
The basic scientists at the State University of New York Upstate
Medical Center, the medical school affiliated with the VA hospital, were
not only uninterested, they were horrified at what I was doing and
wouldn't risk their reputations by becoming associated with me in any
way. So, I walked across the street to the physics department of Syracuse
University and spoke to the chairman, an astronomer whom I'd met a
few years earlier when I volunteered to watch the northern lights during
the International Geophysical Year. After a few minutes' thought, he
suggested that a guy on the third floor named Charlie Bachman might
be "as crazy as you," and wished me luck.
The instant I opened the door, I knew I was in the right place. There
was Charlie, bent over a workbench with an electromagnet and a live
frog.
next-88s
The Circuit of
Awareness
FAIR USE NOTICE
This site contains copyrighted material the use of which has not always been specifically authorized by the copyright owner. As a journalist, I am making such material available in my efforts to advance understanding of artistic, cultural, historic, religious and political issues. I believe this constitutes a 'fair use' of any such copyrighted material as provided for in section 107 of the US Copyright Law.
In accordance with Title 17 U.S.C. Section 107, the material on this site is distributed without profit to those who have expressed a prior interest in receiving the included information for research and educational purposes. Copyrighted material can be removed on the request of the owner.
No comments:
Post a Comment